Citations related to SOCIOLOGY (works cited listed at bottom):
“In Bellah’s account, ‘society’ is, for Durkheim, an immensely
complicated, multilayered reality. Sometimes ‘society’ refers to specific
social groups, but other times it is, in Durkheim’s words, ‘a composition
of ideas, beliefs, and sentiments of all sorts which realize themselves
through individuals. Foremost of these ideas is the moral ideal which is
its principal raison d’etre.’ In Bellah’s words, ‘Not only is society not
identical with an external ‘material entity,’ it is something deeply
inner, since for Durkheim it is the source of morality, personality, and
life itself at the human level.... Durkheim uses the word ‘society’ in
ways closer to classical theology than empirical science.’
“Bellah uses the idea of society in a similarly wide and deep range of
senses. He tends not to focus on specific social groups but, rather, seeks
to explicate the moral sinews at the base of social relations. In Bellah’s
work (and work deply influenced by him)–unlike work more centrally
influenced by Marxist and Weberian traditions–one seldom sees interest
groups in zero-sum conflict. Instead, there are persons misunderstanding
or failing to understand (in part, because they hold more or less wealth
or power but also because they simply do not have a requisite conceptual
vocabulary) their fundamental interdependence. Like Durkheim, Bellah’s aim
is to deepen this common understanding of interdependence and to awaken
the sense of mutual responsibility that this entails.” Madsen, Richard.
“Comparative Cosmopolis: Discovering Different Paths to Moral Integration
in the Modern Ecumene.” pp. 105-123. Madsen, Richard et al. Meaning and
Modernity: Religion Polity and Self. University of California Press. 2002.
p. 107.
"I sit in a room without windows, participating in a ritual etched into
twentieth-century tribal memory. I have been here thousands of times
before, literally. I am in a meeting, trying to solve a problem. Using
whatever analytic tool somebody has just read about or been taught at
their most recent training experience, we are trying to come to grips with
a difficult situation. Perhaps it is poor employee morale or productivity.
Or production schedules. Or the redesign of a function. The topic doesn't
matter. What matters is how familiar and terrible our process is for
coming to terms with the complaint.
"The room is adrift in flip chart paper--clouds of lists, issues,
schedules, plans, accountabilities--crudely taped to the wall. They crack
and rustle, fall loose, and, finally, are pulled off the walls, tightly
rolled, and transported to some innocent secretary, who will litter the
floor around her desk so that, peering down from her keyboard, she can
transcribe them to tidy sheets, which she will mail to us. They will
appear on our desks days or weeks later, faint specters of commitments and
plans, devoid of even the little energy and clarity that sent the original
clouds--poof--up onto the wall. They will drift into our day planners and
onto individual 'to do' lists, lists already fogged with confusion and
inertia. Whether they get 'done' or not, they will not solve the problem.
"I am weary of the lists we make, the time projections we spin out, the
breaking apart and putting back together of problems. It does not work.
The lists and charts we make do not capture experience. They only tell of
our desire to control a reality that is slippery and evasive and
perplexing beyond comprehension. Like bewildered shamans, we perform
rituals passed down to us hoping they will perform miracles. No new wisdom
teacher has appeared to show us how to fit more comfortably into the
universe. Our world grows more disturbing and mysterious, our failures to
predict and control leer back at us from many places, yet to what else can
we turn? If the world is not linear, then our approaches cannot work. But
then, where are we?...
"Several years ago, organizational theorist Karl Weick called attention to
enactment in organizations--how we participate in the creation of
organizational realities. 'The environment that the organization worries
about is put there by the organization,' he observed, adding that if we
acknowledge the role we play in this creation, it changes the things we
talk and argue about. If we create the environment, how can we argue about
its objective features, or about what's true or false? Instead, Weick
encouraged us to focus our concerns on issues of effectiveness, on
questions of what happened, and what actions might have served us better.
We could stop arguing about truth and get on with figuring what works
best.
"Weick also suggested a new approach to organization analysis. Acting
should precede planning, he said, because it is only through action and
implementation that we create the environment. Until we put the
environment in place, how can we formulate our thoughts and plans? In
strategic planning, we act as though we are responding to a demand from
the environment; but, in fact, Weick argued, we create the environment
through our own strong intentions. Strategies should be 'just-in-time...,
supported by more investment in general knowledge, a large skill
repertoire, the ability to do a quick study, trust in intuitions, and
sophistication in cutting losses.'"...
"The participatory nature of reality has focused scientific attention on
relationships. Nothing exists at the subatomic level, or can be observed,
without engagement with another energy source. This focus on relationships
is also a dominant theme in today's management advice. For many years, the
prevailing maxim of management stated: 'Management is getting work done
through others.' The important thing was the work; the 'others' were
nuisances that needed to be managed into conformity and predictability.
Managers have recently been urged to notice that they have people working
for them. They have been advised that work gets done by humans like
themselves, each with strong desires for recognition and connectedness.
The more they (we) feel part of the organization, the more work gets done.
"This, of course, brings with it a host of new, relationship-based
problems that are receiving much notice. How do we get people to work well
together? How do we honor and benefit from diversity? How do we get teams
working together quickly and efficiently? How do we resolve conflicts?
These relationships are confusing and hard to manage, so much so that
after a few years away from their MBA programs, most managers report that
they wish they had focused more on people management skills while in
school.
"Leadership skills have also taken on a relational slant. Leaders are
being encouraged to include stakeholders, to evoke followership, to
empower others. Earlier, when we focused on tasks, and people were the
annoying inconvenience, we thought about 'situational' leadership--how the
situation could affect our choice of styles. A different understanding of
leadership has emerged recently. Leadership is always dependent on the
context, but the context is established by the relationships we value."
Wheatley, Margaret, Leadership and the New Science; Learning about
Organization from an Orderly Universe, Berrett-Koehler Publishers, 1993,
pp. 25, 37, 144.
"For the Greeks it is not because it is care for others that it is
ethical. Care for self is ethical in itself, but it implies complex
relations with others, in the measure where this ethos of freedom is also
a way of caring for others....
"What I wanted to know was how the subject constituted himself, in such
and such a determined form, as a mad subject or as a normal subject,
through a certain number of practices which were games of truth,
applications of power, etc. I had to reject a certain a priori theory of
the subject in order to make this analysis of the relationships which can
exist between the constitution of the subject or different forms of the
subject and games of truth, practices of power and so forth.
"Question: That means that the subject is not a substance?
"It is not a substance; it is a form and this form is not above all or
always identical to itself. You do not have towards yourself the same kind
of relationships when you constitute yourself as political subject who
goes and votes or speaks up in a meeting, and when you try to fulfill your
desires in a sexual relationship. There are no doubt some relationships
and some interferences between these different kinds of subject but we are
not in the presence of the same kind of subject. In each case, we play, we
establish with one's self some different form of relationship. And it is
precisely the historical constitution of these different forms of subject
relating to games of truth that interest me....
"The thought that there could be a state of communication which would be
such that the games of truth could circulate freely, without obstacles,
without constraint and without coercive effects, seems to me to be Utopia.
It is being blind to the fact that relations of power are not something
bad in themselves, from which one must free one's self. I don't believe
there can be a society without relations of power, if you understand them
as means by which individuals try to conduct, to determine the behavior of
others. The problem is not of trying to dissolve them in the utopia of a
perfectly transparent communication, but to give one's self the rules of
law, the techniques of management, and also the ethics, the ethos, the
practice of self, which would allow these games of power to be played with
a minimum of domination." Interview with Michel Foucault, January, 1984,
Bernauer and Rasmussen, The Final Foucault, MIT Press, 1988, pps 7, 10,
18.
"Researchers negotiate their results with the actors and their method with
the research community. A method must be systematic and explicit, but also
much more: It must be open for debate and the influence of fashions and
politics.
"This is not a common picture of the process, however. The social science
community is guilty of several hypocrisies concerning its own conduct. One
is the claim that there are objective and external criteria of
acceptability; although one might allow for 'pluralism,' one never admits
'politics of meaning.' The result is a strong conviction that a good
method unerringly leads to good results, whereas in fact the two are
negotiated in two different and separate arenas. A good method gives at
least a decent result, but not always a very good one, and sometimes good
results are achieved by a faulty method. The two are loosely coupled.
"Another hypocrisy is that the heroes of the field never follow the
prescriptions of the field. Weber, Marx, and Freud were truly poets;
students are advised to write uninspiring prose. Moreover, students are
encouraged to follow existing methods and schools of thought, whereas the
laurels go to those who abolish them." Czarniawska-Joerges, Barbara,
Exploring Complex Organizations; A Cultural Perspective, Sage
Publications, 1992, p. 221.
"The word culture has many meanings and connotations. When we apply it to
groups and organizations, we are almost certain to have conceptual and
semantic confusion...
"Commonly used words relating to culture emphasize one of its critical
aspects--the idea that certain things in groups are shared or held in
common. The major categories of such overt phenomena that are associated
with culture in this sense are the following:
1. Observed behavioral regularities when people interact: the language
they use, the customs and traditions that evolve, and the rituals they
employ in a wide variety of situations.
2. Group norms: the implicit standards and values that evolve in working
groups, such as the particular norm of 'a fair day's work for a fair day's
pay'...
3. Espoused values: the articulated, publicly announced principles and
values that the group claims to be trying to achieve, such as 'product
quality' or 'price leadership'.
4. Formal philosophy: the broad policies and ideological principles that
guide a group's actions toward stockholders, employees, customers, and
other stakeholders, such as the highly publicized 'HP Way' of
Hewlett-Packard.
5. Rules of the game: the implicit rules for getting along in the
organization, 'the ropes' that a newcomer must learn to become an accepted
member, 'the way we do things around here'.
6. Climate: the feeling that is conveyed in a group by the physical layout
and the way in which members of the organization interact with each other,
with customers, or with other outsiders.
7. Embedded skills: the special competencies group members display in
accomplishing certain tasks, the ability to make certain things that gets
passed on from generation to generation without necessarily being
articulated in writing.
8. Habits of thinking, mental models, and/or linguistic paradigms: the
shared cognitive frames that guide the perceptions, thought, and language
used by the members in the early socialization process.
9. Shared meanings: the emergent understandings that are created by group
members as they interact with each other.
10. 'Root metaphors' or integrating symbols: the ideas, feelings, and
images groups develop to characterize themselves, that may or may not be
appreciated consciously but that become embodied in buildings, office
layout, and other material artifacts of the group. This level of the
culture reflects group members' emotional and aesthetic responses as
contrasted with their cognitive or evaluative response." Schein, Edgar H.,
Organizational Culture and Leadership, Jossey-Bass, Second Edition, 1992,
pp. 7-10.
“Different theorists tend to approach society from one perspective or the
other. Someone like John Rawls would, implicitly at least, consider
society from a lifeworld perspective, looking for the overlapping
consensus that participants in a political community might share and
communicatively reproduce. Conversely, theorists such as Emile Durkheim
and Niklas Luhmann adopt, indeed develop, the systems-theoretic approach,
‘realistically’ looking at the constraints and imperatives that various
subsystems impose upon social actors....
“Yet even as he integrates both the lifeworld and the system perspective
into his analysis, Habermas notices that there has been an increasing
differentiation or decoupling between the system and life-world aspects of
society.” McAfee, Noelle. Habermas, Kristeva, and Citizenship. Cornell U.
Press. 2000. p. 86-7.
“Noise is always in formation; there can be neither form nor formation
without noise. When information is understood as a process rather than a
product, the line separating it from noise is difficult to determine.
Noise is not absolute but is relative to the systems it disrupts and
reconfigures, and, conversely, information is not fixed and stable but is
always forming and reforming in relation to noise. Forever parasitic,
noise is the static that prevents the systems it haunts from becoming
static. Static makes systems shifty. If, on the one hand, structures
become too rigid to adapt to changing circumstances, the systems they
support collapse; if, on the other hand, there are no systems to process
data, noise becomes fatal. Life is lived on the shifting margin, boundary,
edge between order and chaos, difference and indifference, negentropy and
entropy, information and noise. The interplay of noise, which is
informative, and information, which is noisy, creates the conditions for
emerging complexity, which is the pulse of life.” Taylor, Mark C. The
Moment of Complexity: Emerging Network Culture. University of Chicago.
2001. p. 123.
“Insofar as myths and the networks of symbols comprising them function as
complex adaptive systems, they form something like what E. O. Wilson
labels a ‘superorganism,’ which is both independent of and lives in and
through individual minds.” Taylor, Mark C. The Moment of Complexity:
Emerging Network Culture. University of Chicago. 2001. p. 214.
“Because the human mode of cultural organization is so distinctive when
compared with those of others animal species, because raising nonhuman
animals within a cultural context does not magically transform them into
human-like cultural beings, and because there are some humans with
biological deficits who do not participate fully in their cultures, the
ineluctable conclusion is that individual human beings possess a
biologically inherited capacity for living culturally.” Tomasello,
Michael. The Cultural Origins of Human Cognition. Harvard University
Press. 1999. p. 53.
“‘Grid’ refers to the system of classification used in a society to
determine roles and statuses, rights and responsibilities. A society with
‘strong grid’ is highly regulated, with strong institutions. Its system of
rules and distinctions is comprehensive in scope and highly coherent in
form. Everyone has a specified place and faces clearly defined
regulations. A society with ‘weak grid’ is one in which the rules and
classifications are indistinct and/or in which clearly defined rules only
apply to limited sectors of life.
“By ‘group,’ Douglas refers to the capacity of a society to put social
pressure on its members. This capacity is strongest, obviously, when
persons are confined to small groups with strong boundaries, so that they
cannot escape scrutiny from and interaction with other group members. This
is what Douglas calls ‘strong group.’ In contrast, the capacity to exert
social pressure is weakest when people live in large, open, mobile
societies where they can voluntarily choose associates. This is ‘weak
group.’” Madsen, Richard. “Comparative Cosmopolis: Discovering Different
Paths to Moral Integration in the Modern Ecumene.” pp. 105-123. Madsen,
Richard & William Sullivan, Ann Swidler, Steven Tipton. Meaning and
Modernity: Religion Polity and Self. University of California Press. 2002.
p. 113.
“There is agreement within the discipline [of sociology] today that the
point of departure for all systems-theoretical analysis must be the
difference between system and environment. Systems are oriented by their
environment not just occasionally and adaptively, but structurally, and
they cannot exist without an environment. They constitute and maintain
themselves by creating and maintaining a difference from their
environment, and they use their boundaries to regulate this difference.
Without difference from an environment, there would not even be
self-reference, because difference is the functional premise of
self-referential operations. In this sense, boundary maintenance is system
maintenance.” Luhmann, Niklas. “The Autopoiesis of Social Systems” in
Essays on Self-Reference. Columbia University Press. 1990. Quoted in:
Taylor, Mark C. The Moment of Complexity: Emerging Network Culture.
University of Chicago. 2001. p. 91.
“In every case, including Europe but more seriously outside of Europe,
development destroys, more or less rapidly, local solidarities and
original traits adapted to specific ecological conditions.
“One must not, of course, idealize cultures. One must recognize that all
evolutions involve leaving something behind, that all creation involves
destruction, and that every historical gain is paid for with a loss.”
Morin, Edgar. Homeland Earth: A Manifesto for a New Millennium. 1999.
Hampton Press. p. 62.
"A long time ago when cultures were more stable, initiation of young men
was carried out by the fathers' and uncles' laying a hand on the shoulder
and saying 'You're a man. Go out and do what you want.' Today the
situation is reversed. Now that the chain of initiation from fathers to
sons is broken, young men spend a lifetime in pursuits trying to prove
that they are men, to gain acceptance. Before it was acceptance and then
the pursuit of the activities of life. Now it is an endless pursuit of
activities in the vain quest for acceptance." Paraphrased from Bly,
Robert, poet, Mendocino, 1983.
“The notion that culture is transmissible from one generation to the next
as a corpus of knowledge, independently of its application in the world,
is untenable for the simple reason that it rests on the impossible
precondition of a ready-made cognitive architecture. In fact, I maintain,
nothing is really transmitted at all. The growth of knowledge in the life
history of a person is a result not of information transmission but of
guided rediscovery, where what each generation contributes to the next are
not rules and representations for the production of appropriate behavior
but the specific conditions of development under which successors, growing
up in a social world, can build up their own aptitudes and dispositions.”
Oyama, Susan et al, ed. Cycles of Contingency: Developmental Systems and
Evolution. MIT Press. 2001. Ingold, Tim. “From Complementarity to
Obviation.” P. 272.
“... social systems are ‘designs for living in particular environments’,
while Culture, along with history and the conditions of the environment,
is what causes social systems. Methods of subsistence, settlement
patterns, even the relationships in which people engage, are all aspects
of the social system, which is the embodiment of the interaction between
Culture, history and environment.” Plotkin, Henry. The Imagined World Made
Real: Towards a Natural Science of Culture. Rutgers University Press.
2003. P. 103.
“Culture is reserved for that something shared through learning. Culture,
to be sure, gives rise to material objects, such as stone tools or
clothes, immaterial objects, such as United Nations resolutions or acts of
parliament, and social organizations, like schools and labour unions. In
short, Culture has consequences, and those consequences, out there in the
world, are commonly referred to as ‘social this’ and ‘social that.’”
Plotkin, Henry. The Imagined World Made Real: Towards a Natural Science of
Culture. Rutgers University Press. 2003. P. 104.
“At the heart of this is the notion that humans have a genetic
predisposition to choose between cultural variants, be these beliefs,
attitudes, values, specific overt behaviours or anything else that can be
culturally transmitted, on the basis of the frequency with which they
occur in a group or population. This bias must not simply reflect the
relative frequency of occurrence of a cultural variant. Rather the naive
individual must be disproportionately likely to choose and act on the
variant which is more common.” Plotkin, Henry. The Imagined World Made
Real: Towards a Natural Science of Culture. Rutgers University Press.
2003. Pps. 240-1.
“Both Bourdieu and Giddens argue that people’s daily routines are rooted
in a taken-for-granted world. In general, people know how to act in
accordance with the implicit, shared rules which make up that world. They
draw upon these rules, and in so doing they unintentionally reproduce
them.” Baert, Patrick. Social Theory in the Twentieth Century. New York
University Press. 1998. P. 4.
“In other words, we do what we do in part because of the position we
occupy in our surrounding social structure and in part because of our
innate preferences and characteristics. In sociology, these two forces are
called structure and agency, and the evolution of a social network is
driven by a trade-off between the two. Because agency is the part of an
individual’s decision-making process that is not constrained by his or her
structural position, actions derived from agency appear as random events
to the rest of the world.” Watts, Duncan. Six Degrees: The Science of a
Connected Age. 2003. W.W. Norton. P. 72.
“Strictly
speaking, we observe transactions, not relations. Transactions between
social sites transfer energy from one to another, however microscopically.
From a series of transactions we infer a relations between the sites: a
friendship, a rivalry, an alliance, or something else.” Tilly, Charles.
Identities, Boundaries, & Social Ties. 2005. Paradigm Publishers. P. 7.
“Overall, it [a figure diagram] analyzes the transformation of collective
identities: shared answers to the questions ‘Who are you?’ ‘Who are we?’
or ‘Who are they?’ Such identities, it indicates, center on boundaries
separating us from them. On either side of the boundary, people maintain
relations with each other: relations within X and relations within Y. They
also carry on relations across the boundary: relations linking X to Y.
Finally, the create collective stories about the boundary, about relations
within X and Y, and relations between X and Y. Those stories usually
differ from one side of the boundary to another, and often influence each
other. Together, boundary, cross-boundary relations, within-boundary
relations, and stories make up collective identities. Changes in any of
the elements, however they occur, affect all the others. The existence of
collective identities, furthermore, shapes individual experiences, for
example, by providing templates for us Croats and distinguishing us from
those Serbs.” Tilly, Charles. Identities, Boundaries, & Social Ties. 2005.
Paradigm Publishers. Pp. 7-8.
“Crudely speaking, general descriptions and explanations of social
processes divide into three categories; systemic, dispositional, and
transactional. Systemic accounts posit a coherent, self-sustaining entity
such as a society, a world-economy, a community, an organization, a
household, or at the limit a person, explaining events inside that entity
by their location within the entity as a whole. Systemic descriptions and
explanations have the advantage of taking seriously a knotty problem for
social scientists: how to connect small-scale and large-scale social
processes. They have two vexing disadvantages: the enormous difficulty of
identifying and bounding relevant systems, and persistent confusion about
cause and effect within such systems.
“Dispositional accounts similarly posit coherent entities–in this case
more often individuals than any others–but explain the actions of those
entities by means of their orientations just before the point of action.
Competing dispositional accounts feature motives, decision logics,
emotions, and cultural templates. When cast at the level of the individual
organism, dispositional descriptions and explanations have the advantage
of articulating easily with the findings of neuroscience, genetics, and
evolutionary analysis. They have the great disadvantage of accounting
badly for emergence and for aggregate effects.
“Transactional accounts take interactions among social sites as their
starting points, treating both events at those sites and durable
characteristics of those sites as outcomes of interactions. Transactional
accounts become relational–another term widely employed in this
context–when they focus on persistent features of transactions between
specific social sites. Transactional or relational descriptions and
explanations have the advantage of placing communication, including the
use of language, at the heart of social life. They have the disadvantage
of contradicting common sense accounts of social behavior, and thus of
articulating poorly with conventional moral reasoning in which entities
take responsibility for dispositions and their consequences.
“Systemic, dispositional, and transactional approaches qualify as
metatheories rather than as directly verifiable or falsifiable theories.
They take competing ontological positions, claiming that rather different
sorts of phenomena constitute and cause social processes. In the nature of
the case, however, sustained competition between social scientific
explanations usually takes place within one of these ontological lines
rather than across them; systemic explanations compete with other systemic
explanations, and so on.” Tilly, Charles. Identities, Boundaries, & Social
Ties. 2005. Paradigm Publishers. Pp. 14-5.
“As represented by manuals, courses, and presidential addresses, approved
social science doctrine generally favors some combination of dispositional
and covering law explanations: to explain political action means not only
to reconstruct accurately the state of an actor–especially, but not
exclusively, intentions of a cogitating individual–at the point of action,
but to locate that state as a special case of a general law concerning
human behavior. Such a doctrine rests on an implausible claim: that
ultimately all political processes result from extremely general
uniformities in the propensities of human actors, especially individual
actors. Despite more than a century of strenuous effort, social scientists
have securely identified no such uniformities. But they have, in fact,
recurrently identified widely operating causal mechanisms and processes.
Rather than continuing to search for disposition-governing covering laws,
it would therefore make sense to switch wholeheartedly toward
specification of mechanisms and processes.” Tilly, Charles. Identities,
Boundaries, & Social Ties. 2005. Paradigm Publishers. Pp. 27-8.
“Less obviously, democracy depends on a degree of articulation between
public politics and networks of interpersonal commitment organized around
trade, religious practice, kinship, and esoteric knowledge. In networks of
these kinds, people carry on long-term, high-risk activities whose
outcomes depend significantly on the performances of others; in that
sense, they qualify as trust networks.” Tilly, Charles. Identities,
Boundaries, & Social Ties. 2005. Paradigm Publishers. P. 55.
“Social ties entailing significant rights and obligations grow up from a
wide variety of activities: birth, common residence, sexual relations,
mutual aid, religious practice, public ceremonial, and more. Some of those
ties ramify into networks of identity and trust: sets of social ties
providing collective answers to the questions ‘Who are you?’ ‘Who are we?’
and ‘Who are they?’ as well as becoming sites of high-risk, long-term
activities such as reputation building, investment, trade in valuables,
procreation, and entrance into a craft. Trade diasporas, Landsmannschaften,
credit circles, lineages, religious sects, and journeymen’s brotherhoods
provide salient cases in point.
“Networks thus formed and reinforced acquire strong claims over their
members. Gossip, shaming, and threats of expulsion multiply their
effectiveness in such networks. Since the very connections among members
become crucial resources, external threats to any member become threats to
the high-risk, long-term activities of all members. As a result, external
repression operates on networks of identity and trust in two rather
different modes. It damps collective action when it concentrates on
raising the cost of any new action but incites collective resistance when
it threatens survival of the network and its associated identities.” Tilly,
Charles. Identities, Boundaries, & Social Ties. 2005. Paradigm Publishers.
P. 57.
“Participation in contentious politics consists of conversational
interaction within networks in the context of collectively constructed
stories.” Tilly, Charles. Identities, Boundaries, & Social Ties. 2005.
Paradigm Publishers. P. 58.
“Scripts provide models for participation in particular classes of social
relations. Shared local knowledge, in its turn, provides means of giving
variable contents to those social relations. Among our basic mechanisms,
emulation relies chiefly on scripting, while adaptation relies heavily on
accumulation of local knowledge.” Tilly, Charles. Identities, Boundaries,
& Social Ties. 2005. Paradigm Publishers. P. 81.
“Scripts alone promote uniformity; knowledge alone flexibility; their
combination, flexibility within established limits.
“With little scripting and local knowledge available, actors either avoid
each other or follow shallow improvisations such as the maneuvers
pedestrians on a crowded sidewalk adopt in order to pass each other with a
minimum of bumping and blocking. Scripting can be extensive and common
knowledge meager, as when a master of ceremonies directs participants to
applaud, rise, sit, and exit; let us entitle this circumstance thin
ritual. Here only weak ties obtain. Thin ritual absorbs high transaction
costs for the social results that it accomplishes; most people reserve it
for very special occasions, and escape it when they can.” Tilly, Charles.
Identities, Boundaries, & Social Ties. 2005. Paradigm Publishers. P. 81.
“Wherever powerful parties gain from the segregation and coordination of
two networks, equal or not, paired categories provide an effective device
for realization of that gain.” Tilly, Charles. Identities, Boundaries, &
Social Ties. 2005. Paradigm Publishers. P.87.
"I shall discuss two concepts of social order: that of stable, regular,
predictable patterns of behaviour and that of cooperative behaviour.
Correspondingly, there are two concepts of disorder. The first, disorder
as lack of predictability, is expressed in Macbeth's vision of life as
'sound and fury, a tale told by an idiot, signifying nothing'. The second,
disorder as absence of cooperation, is expressed in Hobbes's vision of
life in the state of nature as 'solitary, poor, nasty, brutish, and
short'....
"What is it that enables people to predict each other's behaviour? What is
it that enables them to cooperate with one another? The partial answers I
have provided are several sizes smaller than the questions. altruism,
envy, social norms and self-interest all contribute, in complex,
interacting ways to order, stability and cooperation. Some mechanisms that
promote stability also work against cooperation. Some mechanisms that
facilitate cooperation also increase the level of violence. Each society
and each community will be glued together, for better and for worse, by a
particular, idiosyncratic mix of these motivations. But the basic
ingredients that go into the cement seem to be more or less the same in
all societies, even if they can be combined in innumerable ways." Elster,
Jon. The Cement of Society: A Study of Social Order. Cambridge University
Press. 1989. Pp. 1, 287.
“In this oeuvre [actant-network theory], which emerged from new
theorisations of the relations between people and technology, objects are
constructed by particular power relations, and in turn also actively
construct such relations. In this tradition, known as actant-network
theory, objects are produced by particular networks of cultural and
political discourses and, in conjunction with humans, act to reproduce
such relations. So, the discourses and networks which connect people to
objects are not only inextricable as if they are one actor, but may in
fact be ‘made of the same stuff.’” Woodward, Ian. Understanding Material
Culture. 2007. Sage Publications. P. 12. Subquote is from MacKenzie and
Wajcman, The Social Shaping of Technology. 1999, Open University Press, P.
25.
“‘Actant’ is a term developed from recent approaches in the sociology of
science and technology which refers to entities – both human and non-human
– which have the ability to ‘act’ socially. By dissolving the boundary
between people who ‘act’ and objects which are seen as inanimate or
‘outside’, the term ‘actant’ is designed to overcome any a priori
distinction between the social, technological and natural worlds, and
emphasises the inextricable links between humans and material things.”
Woodward, Ian. Understanding Material Culture. 2007. Sage Publications. P.
15.
“Identities, which are the nodes, trigger out of struggles for control as
they seek footing with each other, and so co-evolve along with networks in
one and another tangible domain of activity.” White, Harrison. Identity
and Control: How Social Formations Emerge, Second Edition. 2008. Princeton
University Press. P. xviii.
“A higher-level network can grow, for example, among nodes that are
disciplines. A style is itself recognizable as a new level, an identity
with a new sort of internal constitution. The publics induced and
presupposed in constructing identities and networks in chapters 1 and 2,
can also be seen as a zero level. The possibilities are myriad, and
dizzying, as indeed they must be for an accounting of our vast ‘river.’”
White, Harrison. Identity and Control: How Social Formations Emerge,
Second Edition. 2008. Princeton University Press. P. xix.
“If we assume with Luhmann that all events are fugitive and that they are
the elements of social systems, then control becomes the attempt to
constrain the possible events.” White, Harrison. Identity and Control: How
Social Formations Emerge, Second Edition. 2008. Princeton University
Press. Note on P. 7.
“Triggerings of identities also invoke communication with others as an
aspect of seeking footings. Repeated communication between some pair can
get recognized as a continuing relation when its frequency rises above
chance expectancy in that context. The pattern of such ties across
identities can become seen as a network engraved in some sort of public
space with an identity of its own. Influences flowing through ties, and
their impacts, are shaped by the network and in turn can reshape it.
“Baldassarri and Bearman model how this may transpire when political
issues that are already active in participants’ minds are the subjects of
communications. Repeated communication will include arguments and attempts
at influencing the opinion of the other participant in a tie. A threefold
stochastic model is proposed for showing how the choice of what issue to
discuss, and with whom, shift along with the existing divergence of
opinion. Baldassarri and Bearman ran large numbers of massive simulations
of the resulting distribution and location of opinions in that public,
across the menu of issues.
“The point is to see how an extreme partisan polarization can come about
even within a public most of whom take moderate stands on most issues. The
crux of the study’s findings is that, even with the majority moderate on
most issues, this public can sift itself into rather segregated and
homogeneously partisan blocs of opinion on one or a few hot-button issues.
The simulations were run over hundreds and thousands of periods of
discussion, and the output provided the full network on each issue at each
period, noting the opinion level of each actor at each stage. Not always,
but often, an issue or two sorted themselves out as hot-button without
attribution of particular content.” White, Harrison. Identity and Control:
How Social Formations Emerge, Second Edition. 2008. Princeton University
Press. P. 21. [Reference is Baladassarri, Delia & Bearman. 2006. “Dynamics
of Political Polarization.” ISERP Working Paper. 06-07. Columbia
University.]
“A story is at root an authority, a transfer of identity, which explains
its binding to network.” White, Harrison. Identity and Control: How Social
Formations Emerge, Second Edition. 2008. Princeton University Press. P.
31.
“Granovetter derived from Rapoport’s results the conclusion that ties and
network were intertwined in a manner that was, at first sight,
paradoxical. Ties that were intrinsically weaker, more casual, yielded
higher connectivity across the network: weak ties are strong. That is, the
way in which weak ties spread themselves around is such that they connect
a larger fraction of a world together than do the same number of strong
ties spread out in their way.
“Strong ties, ties given precedence by the issuers, are weak in the
broader context because they do not bind as large a fraction of a world
into a corporate whole in connectivity. Granovetter elaborated all the
nuances implied. Strong ties did fit into strong, if tiny, corporates so
inwardly turned as only to choose each of the few intimate others again
and again without attention to the larger context of persons. Sum it up
abstractly: Close-knitness of a network is highly correlated with
involuteness.” White, Harrison. Identity and Control: How Social
Formations Emerge, Second Edition. 2008. Princeton University Press. Pp.
43-4. [Reference is Granovetter, Mark. 1973. “The Strength of Weak Ties.”
American Journal of Sociology 78:1360-1380.]
“James Coleman reports on corporates with equal richness from a smaller
and more specialized canvas, the American high school. In Coleman’s
account, it is initial networks among youngsters feeding in from diverse
elementary schools and family clusters, which are overtaken by corporates
that emerge among the children in straggly fashion. Coleman’s substantive
theme is the preoccupations and machinations of identities situated in
these networks to become assimilated to the ‘right’ sorts of corporates.
These are the ‘in’ crowds on a social level, specializing variously around
clothes and clubs and hangouts and sports and so on. Of course, there can
be more or less bullying too, correlating with differences in
architectures across corporates.” White, Harrison. Identity and Control:
How Social Formations Emerge, Second Edition. 2008. Princeton University
Press. P. 46. [Reference is Coleman, James. 1961. The Adolescent Society.
Free Press.]
“Structural equivalence is a more general concept than membership or
network. It includes as a special case, but may be contrasted with, the
cohesion of corporate interconnection . It concerns mutual positioning:
what partition into sets of identities would signal what partition of
types of tie? Note the duality. Blocks of structurally equivalent
identities are built according to tie profiles. For an explicit
definition, consult Breiger, Boorman, and Arabie.
“There may be no ties at all between structural equivalents. Two lonely
kids alike isolated on the fringes of a playground illustrate the
pervasiveness of marginality in networks. Romo analyzes this as the ‘Omega
Phenomenon.’ Also structurally equivalent are two ‘stars’ who each reach
out to gather the other kids into their respective orbits but have little
to do with each other. Or structural equivalence can be abstracted from
the particular others, so that two quarterbacks are equivalent even though
there is no overlap between the kids in their orbits. The result is
positions.
“The central point is to look for a partition of a population, such that
the nodes in each set tend to relate to the rest of the sets in much the
same way: in the pure case, they have the same incidence of the same sort
of ties into each other set. According to this principle, just call it
streq, those in a set see the rest of the world the same way but need not
even be aware of each other, much less be tied as a clique.” White,
Harrison. Identity and Control: How Social Formations Emerge, Second
Edition. 2008. Princeton University Press. P. 54. [Reference is to Breiger,
Ronald, Boorman & Arabie. “An Algorithm for Clustering Relational Data
with Applications to Social Network Analysis and Comparison with
Multidimensional Scaling.” 1975. Journal of Mathematical Psychology
12:328-83.]
“Institutions and rhetorics are akin to networks and stories, in that
spaces of possibilities for the ordinary in life, of what will be taken
for granted, derive from each pair. Rhetorics make institutions explicit
just as stories make networks explicit. Rhetoric is the garb of a realm,
much as story-set clothes type of tie [sic]. An institutional system has
come to accommodate a wide range of disciplines and styles as well as
networks within a realm, along with the particular institutions. They are
gathered together with rhetorics that constitute that realm. And yet
institutional systems also precede and influence, as well as build from,
these constituents. Within each system, stories must continue to accompany
local enclaves at the scale of disciplines and yet be configured so as to
transpose across network populations and styles. The stories become
mutually shared accounts when they muster through publics into rhetorics.”
White, Harrison. Identity and Control: How Social Formations Emerge,
Second Edition. 2008. Princeton University Press. Pp. 171-2.
“So language is coordinate to processes that lead to stories and to types
of tie being discerned and factored out as separate patterns, networks.
Only the human species elaborates ties in stories. Social accountings
ground social networks in a somehow-ordered heap of stories, only some of
whose constituents map into biophysical space. The generation and spread
of these accountings presuppose social contexts able to support language
as structuring.” White, Harrison. Identity and Control: How Social
Formations Emerge, Second Edition. 2008. Princeton University Press. P.
343.
“Socially established communication patterns are identified by social
systems theory as the individual elements that constitute society. Society
consists of social systems, of certain communicational ‘organisms’ that
emerged and have established their own specific types of operations. These
can connect to each other and continue the operations of this
communicational organism–similar to a cell that by its bio-chemical
operations creates its own autopoietic ‘being.’ A social system ‘is’
nothing else but the autopoietic reproduction of itself. A social system,
such as the economy, consists of nothing but economic communication that
connects to itself. It is only by economic communication that the economy
continues and further constructs itself–and thus builds its own
communicational ‘membrane’ by which economic communication can be
distinguished from other types of communication.” Moeller, Hans-Georg.
Luhmann Explained: From Souls to Systems. 2006. Open Court Publishing. Pp.
23-4.
“According to social systems theory, society has evolved to a state in
which it consists of a variety of large communication systems that can be
identified by the functions they perform. Such function systems are, for
instance, economy, politics, law, and mass media: buying a meal is
communication functioning economically; the casting and counting of a vote
is communication functioning politically; presenting an argument in court
is communication functioning legally. Since all these systems are
operationally closed, they are the intrasocial environment of the others.
They are ‘subsystems’ of society. Each function system has its own social
perspective and creates its own social reality.” Moeller, Hans-Georg.
Luhmann Explained: From Souls to Systems. 2006. Open Court Publishing. P.
24.
“In the terminology of social systems theory, function systems can be
identified by their respective codes. The legal system, for instance,
operates on the basis of the legal/illegal code.” Moeller, Hans-Georg.
Luhmann Explained: From Souls to Systems. 2006. Open Court Publishing. P.
25.
“In order to be actually communicated, a code cannot do without programs.
In the system of science, to give another example, the code of true/false
on which scientific communication is based, must be applied in connection
with certain scientific theories, methods, and so on. Without relation to
these programs you cannot argue scientifically for something to be true of
false. Only in the context of, for instance, a theory or a method, can a
scientific statement be called scientifically true or false.” Moeller,
Hans-Georg. Luhmann Explained: From Souls to Systems. 2006. Open Court
Publishing. P. 25.
“Luhmann says in regard to organizations: ‘As a result there comes into
being an autopoietic system that is characterized by a specific form of
operations: It produces decisions by decisions. Behavior is communicated
as decision-making.’ The importance of organizations and their specific
type of communicative operation–decision making–seems to increase in
modern society.” Moeller, Hans-Georg. Luhmann Explained: From Souls to
Systems. 2006. Open Court Publishing. P. 32.
“There is a coupling between politics and law regulated by constitutions.
The economy is coupled to the legal system by ownership and contracts. The
coupling between the system of science and the system of education is
manifested in the organization of the university. Education and the
economy are coupled through academic certificates and diplomas that
regulate access to jobs.” Moeller, Hans-Georg. Luhmann Explained: From
Souls to Systems. 2006. Open Court Publishing. P. 38.
“Structural coupling establishes specific mechanisms of irritation between
systems and forces different systems to continuously resonate with each
other.” Moeller, Hans-Georg. Luhmann Explained: From Souls to Systems.
2006. Open Court Publishing. P. 38.
“Social evolution in the strict sense takes place, according to Luhmann,
when a new type of differentiation becomes dominant.” Moeller, Hans-Georg.
Luhmann Explained: From Souls to Systems. 2006. Open Court Publishing. P.
42.
“This passage makes reference to all four types of differentiation
discussed by Luhmann: segmentary differentiation, center/periphery
differentiation, stratified differentiation, and functional
differentiation.” Moeller, Hans-Georg. Luhmann Explained: From Souls to
Systems. 2006. Open Court Publishing. P. 42
“In a society strictly based on segmentary differentiation, there is no
center of social power–no tribe or segment is generally perceived to be
the core–and there is also no established social hierarchy that has gained
primacy over these structures.” Moeller, Hans-Georg. Luhmann Explained:
From Souls to Systems. 2006. Open Court Publishing. Pp. 42-3.
“The increasing power and wealth of one social segment can lead to an
overturning of segmentary differentiation. One segment may become so
dominant that it establishes the difference between itself and the other
segments as the new primary difference of this society. In this case, a
center/periphery differentiation would be born–and it would be born by the
center itself. The center makes itself the center. An example of a society
with a significant center/periphery distinction would be ancient Rome.”
Moeller, Hans-Georg. Luhmann Explained: From Souls to Systems. 2006. Open
Court Publishing. P. 44.
“In a society based on stratified differentiation, social order is
perceived to be a direct outcome of distinctions in social status. The
subsystems of a stratified society are the different ‘classes’ that
constitute the social hierarchy.” Moeller, Hans-Georg. Luhmann Explained:
From Souls to Systems. 2006. Open Court Publishing. P. 44.
“Contemporary society, as he sees it, is basically the outcome of the
replacement of medieval European stratified differentiation with
functional differentiation between the sixteenth and eighteenth century.”
Moeller, Hans-Georg. Luhmann Explained: From Souls to Systems. 2006. Open
Court Publishing. P. 45.
“Social function systems (politics, economy, education, and so on) are all
‘equally’ different–they all have different codes, programs, media, and so
on but their functional inequality does not go along with a hierarchical
inequality. Like the segments of a segmentary society, function systems
are neither ranked nor oriented towards one central core. But, like the
strata of a stratified society, function systems have separate and
mutually exclusive characteristics. While segmentary differentiation is
based on the structural equality of its subsystems and center/periphery
and stratified differentiations are based on the structural inequality of
their subsystems, the subsystems of functional differentiation are equally
unequal.” Moeller, Hans-Georg. Luhmann Explained: From Souls to Systems.
2006. Open Court Publishing. Pp. 45-6.
“‘Systems operating within the medium of sense can and must distinguish
beteen self-reference and other-reference, and this has to be done so that
the actualization of self-reference also implies other-reference, and that
the actualization of other-reference also implies self-reference as the
respective other side of the distinction.’
“Making sense within a horizon constituted of actuality and possibility
implies the distinction between the sense maker and that which makes sense
for the sense maker–the distinction between the ship and its horizon. A
mind that makes sense can distinguish between itself and what it intends.
Similarly, communication can distinguish between itself and its context.
The expression ‘to make sense’ has a double meaning and always introduces
two elements at the same time: this makes sense to me. It makes sense, and
I make it make sense. The form of sense therefore goes along with two
basic and interconnected structural distinctions: the
actuality/possibility distinction and the self-reference/other-reference
distinction.” Moeller, Hans-Georg. Luhmann Explained: From Souls to
Systems. 2006. Open Court Publishing. P. 67. Subquote is from Luhmann,
Niklas. Die Gesellshaft der Gesellshaft. 1997. Suhrkamp. P. 51.
“Luhmann highlights its [public opinion’s] functional importance. Public
opinion has nothing to do with human subjectivity or with the exercise of
human reason; it is a communicative medium that becomes possible through
the development of the mass media into an autopoietic and global function
system. With the production of a general memory there arises a need for a
‘currency.’ The memory has to somehow take shape; it needs to assume
forms. Like the economy needs the medium of money, so a general reality
needs a medium to manifest itself. Public opinion is this communicative
medium, and it is produced within the mass media system.” Moeller, Hans-Georg.
Luhmann Explained: From Souls to Systems. 2006. Open Court Publishing. P.
138.
“In tighter kinds of sociality, from honeybees to apes, attention to the
attention of others becomes progressively more important. First, animals
read others’ attention to resources as well as risks: a patch of
pollen-rich flowers or ripe fruit, a route to reaching it, a technique to
access or process it, a safe territory. Then, as the advantages of social
living multiply, creatures attend to others in the group and their mutual
attention in order to monitor affiliation and alliance and the rank needed
to avoid constant conflict over access to resources.
“As social relationships become still more flexible, as in corvids,
dolphins, or chimpanzees, shared attention makes possible open-ended
coordination within groups and against rivals or prey. Groups that
coordinate swiftly and freely can outdo other groups. To do so, they need
to share and check attention and intention as they pursue flexible goals:
as Frans de Waal notes, ‘The selection pressure on paying attention to
others must have been enormous.’ And in order to motivate complex
cooperation, animals need to derive interest and pleasure from attending
to each other, as dolphins do in their synchronized play.” Boyd, Brian. On
the Evolution of Stories: Evolution, Cognition, and Fiction. 2009. Harvard
University Press. P. 102.
“Religion and ritual cannot explain the origin of art, but once art began,
traditions of artistic elaboration, including ritual, could be coopted for
social cohesion.” Boyd, Brian. On the Evolution of Stories: Evolution,
Cognition, and Fiction. 2009. Harvard University Press. P. 118.
“Juvenile play deprivation among both rats and humans correlates with
serious social malfunction in later life. Young rats experimentally
deprived of play grow up unable to judge how and when to defend themselves
and veer between being far too aggressive and far too passive. In humans
such experiments have yet to be tried, but in a large-scale study of
sociopathic murderers in Texas, researchers were surprised to find no
common background factor other than an absence or an extremely reduced
amount of play in childhood in 90 percent of the perpetrators.” Boyd,
Brian. On the Evolution of Stories: Evolution, Cognition, and Fiction.
2009. Harvard University Press. Pp. 179-80.
“The first wave of social systems theory is Parson’s structural
functionalism, the second wave is derived from the general systems theory
of the 1960s through the 1990s, and the third wave is based on the complex
dynamical systems theory developed in the 1990s.” Sawyer, R. Keith. Social
Emergence: Societies as Complex Systems. 2005. Cambridge University Press.
P. 10.
“However, the Gestaltists were more holist than emergentist; their
emphasis was on the study of irreducible wholes, and they did not explore
how those wholes emerged from lower-level components and other
interactions.” Sawyer, R. Keith. Social Emergence: Societies as Complex
Systems. 2005. Cambridge University Press. P. 35.
“Most contemporary social theory likewise rejects the historical positions
of both individualism and collectivism. Archer observed that contemporary
attempts at unity have taken two forms: the inseparability and process
ontology of Giddens’s structuration theory and the emergentist and
morphogenetic account of analytic dualism....”
“...Commenting on contemporary social theory, Archer noted that a ‘concern
with interplay is what distinguishes the emergentist from the non-emergentist
whose preoccupation is with interpenetration.’” Sawyer, R. Keith. Social
Emergence: Societies as Complex Systems. 2005. Cambridge University Press.
P. 139.
“Both Castelfranchi and N. Gilbert argued that emergence processes in
systems of cognitive agents are qualitatively different than in reactive
agent systems because cognitive agents are capable of observing and
internalizing emergent macrofeatures of the system, a process that has
been called ‘immergence’ and ‘second-order emergence’” ... Sawyer, R.
Keith. Social Emergence: Societies as Complex Systems. 2005. Cambridge
University Press. P. 172.
“All three of these prominent sociological paradigms [reductions to either
structure, individual, or interaction] fail to theorize the multileveled
nature of society that is brought into focus by artificial societies:
individual, communication, and emergent social properties.” Sawyer, R.
Keith. Social Emergence: Societies as Complex Systems. 2005. Cambridge
University Press. Pp. 187-8.
“The Structure Paradigm focuses on the relations between individuals and
societies. Parsonsian structural-functionalism is the canonical approach
of the Structure Paradigm; this version of the Structure Paradigm was
dominant in the 1950s and 1960s, and other forms of the Structure Paradigm
continue today. The Structure Paradigm was followed by the Interaction
Paradigm. The Interaction Paradigm rejected almost everything central to
the Structure Paradigm and proposed a new alternative not considered
within the Structure Paradigm: that communicative interaction, not the
structure nor the individual, was central to sociological explanation.
“The Emergence Paradigm is a classic synthesis: the inherent tensions of
the Interaction Paradigm drive theory’s movement toward it, and it
combines the central elements of both the Structure Paradigm and the
Interaction Paradigm. The Emergence Paradigm emphasizes both
individual-society relations and communicative interaction, arguing that
the individual-society relation cannot be explained without recourse to
sophisticated theories of communication and of emergence from
communication.” Sawyer, R. Keith. Social Emergence: Societies as Complex
Systems. 2005. Cambridge University Press. Pp. 191-2.
“Interaction can be studied objectively within the positivist tradition,
whereas agency is a subjectivist, interpretivist notion.” Sawyer, R.
Keith. Social Emergence: Societies as Complex Systems. 2005. Cambridge
University Press. P. 207.
“Agency theorists like Taylor and Giddens are ultimately focused on
individuals; they hold that subjective interpretation explains social life
and that there is no need for an autonomous science of society. If
interpretivists are correct, then the social level of analysis does not
exist. Interpretivists have the same attitude about social phenomena as
methodological individualists: Social phenomena do not exist, they are
mere epiphenomena of human action (and humans in interaction)....
“Contemporary interpretivism is a strange and unstable combination of
subjectivist agency theories (e.g., Giddens) and objectivist empirical
studies of interaction (e.g., conversation analysis). Interpretivism is
unstable because it overlaps both agency theory (part of the Structure
Paradigm) and interactionism (part of the Interaction Paradigm).” Sawyer,
R. Keith. Social Emergence: Societies as Complex Systems. 2005. Cambridge
University Press. P. 209.
“As a result of inverted neglects – the Structure Paradigm neglecting
symbolic interaction, the Interaction Paradigm neglecting structural
properties – there has not been any sustained study of the role that
symbolic interaction plays in social emergence.” Sawyer, R. Keith. Social
Emergence: Societies as Complex Systems. 2005. Cambridge University Press.
P. 210.
The Emergence paradigm introduces two additional levels of social reality:
stable emergents and ephemeral emergents. [As levels D and C on a five
level pyramid beginning with individuals at level A, Interaction at B, and
Social Structure at level E]” Sawyer, R. Keith. Social Emergence:
Societies as Complex Systems. 2005. Cambridge University Press. P. 210.
“To the extent that individuals are influenced and constituted by their
social situation, the study of the individual will be a part of the
Emergence Paradigm.” Sawyer, R. Keith. Social Emergence: Societies as
Complex Systems. 2005. Cambridge University Press. P. 223.
“At the most general level, greed is accentuated in human behavior by
civilizations that permit large numbers of people to exist in apparent
‘freedom’ from environmental constraints.” Bennett, John. Human Ecology as
Human Behavior; Essays in Environmental and Developmental Anthropology.
1996. Transaction Publishers. P. 9.
“The term ‘socionatural system’ is an empirical generalization that
attempts to combine both Nature and Culture. Empirically, socionatural
systems can consist of any ongoing relationship between human activities
and environmental phenomena in which the humans provide the goals and
means and the environment the wherewithal.” Bennett, John. Human Ecology
as Human Behavior; Essays in Environmental and Developmental Anthropology.
1996. Transaction Publishers. P. 13.
“The distinctive feature of human social reality, the way in which it
differs from other forms of animal reality known to me, is that humans
have the capacity to impose functions on objects and people where the
objects and the people cannot perform the functions solely in virtue of
their physical structure. The performance of the function requires that
there be a collectively recognized status that the person or object has,
and it is only in virtue of that status that the person or object can
perform the function in question. Examples are pretty much everywhere: a
piece of private property, the president of the United States, a
twenty-dollar bill, and a professor in a university are all people or
objects that are able to perform certain functions in virtue of the fact
that they have a collectively recognized status that enables them to
perform those functions in a way they could not do without the collective
recognition of the status.” Searle, John. Making the Social World: The
Structure of Human Civilization. 2010. Oxford University Press. P. 7.
“It is important to distinguish at least two kinds of rules. Our favorite
examples of rules regulate antecedently existing forms of behavior. For
example, the rule ‘Drive on the right-hand side of the road’ regulates
driving in the United States, but driving can exist independently of this
rule. Some rules, however, do not just regulate, but they also create the
possibility of the very behavior that they regulate. So the rules of
chess, for example, do not just regulate pushing pieces around on a board,
but acting in accordance with a sufficient number of the rules is a
logically necessary condition for playing chess, because chess does not
exist apart from the rules. Characteristically, regulative rules have the
form ‘Do X,’ constitutive rules have the form ‘X counts as Y in context
C.’ Thus, for example, such and such counts as a legal knight move in a
game of chess, such and such a position counts as checkmate.” Searle,
John. Making the Social World: The Structure of Human Civilization. 2010.
Oxford University Press. Pp. 9-10.
“Some facts exist independently of any human institution. I call these
brute facts. But some facts require human institutions in order to exist
at all. An example of a brute fact is that the Earth is 93 million miles
from the sun, and an example of an institutional fact is that Barack Obama
is president of the United States. Institutional facts are typically
objective facts, but oddly enough, they are only facts by human agreement
of acceptance. Such facts require institutions for their existence.”
Searle, John. Making the Social World: The Structure of Human
Civilization. 2010. Oxford University Press. P. 10.
“An institution is a system of constitutive rules, and such a system
automatically creates the possibility of institutional facts.” Searle,
John. Making the Social World: The Structure of Human Civilization. 2010.
Oxford University Press. P. 10.
“Basic to that formal structure is the distinction between the ‘cognitive
faculties’ – perception, memory, and belief–and the ‘conative and
volitional faculties’ – desire, prior intention, and intention-in-action.
These two sets relate to reality in quite different ways. I have already
introduced the notion of direction of fit as a feature of speech acts, but
I hope it is obvious that it applies equally well to mental states.
Beliefs, like statements, have the downward or mind (or word)-to-world
direction of fit. And desires and intentions, like orders and promises,
have the upward or world-to-mind (or word) direction of fit. Beliefs and
perceptions, like statements, are supposed to represent how things are in
the world, and in that sense they are supposed to fit the world; they have
mind-to-world direction of fit. The conative-volitional states such as
desires, prior intentions, and intentions-in-action, like orders and
promises, have the world-to-mind direction of fit. They are not supposed
to represent how things are but how we would like them to be or how we
intend to make them be. In addition to these two faculties, there is a
third, imagination, where the propositional content is not supposed to fit
reality in the way that the propositional contents in cognition and
volition are supposed to fit, but which nonetheless functions crucially in
creating social and institutional reality.” Searle, John. Making the
Social World: The Structure of Human Civilization. 2010. Oxford University
Press. P. 15.
“Human beings, along with certain other species, have the capacity to
impose functions on objects, where the imposition of function creates an
intentionality-relative phenomenon, the function. Typically an object will
have a function imposed on it when the object is used for a certain
purpose. I call these ‘agentive functions.’ Humans create agentive
functions to a rather spectacular extent with all of their tools. But even
nonhuman animals can have objects that perform certain functions where the
function is intended by the users of the object: think of birds’ nests,
beaver dams, and primates using a stick to dig food out of the ground. For
our present purposes, it is important to point out that functions are
always intentionality-relative. This is disguised from us by the fact that
in biology we often discover functions in nature. We discover, for
example, that the function of the heart is to pump blood (something that
was unknown until the seventeenth century), or that the function of the
vestibular ocular reflex is to stabilize the retinal image. But when we
discover functions in nature, what we are doing is discovering how certain
causes operate to serve certain purposes, where the notion of purpose is
not intrinsic to mind-independent nature, but is relative to our sets of
values. So we can discover that the heart pumps blood, but when we say
that the function of the heart is to pump blood, we take it for granted
that life, survival, and reproduction are positive values, and that the
functioning of biological organs serves these values. But where do the
values come from? The clue that there is a normative component to the
notion of function is that once we have described something in terms of
function we can introduce a normative vocabulary. We can say things like,
‘This is a better heart than that heart,’ ‘This heart is malfunctioning,’
‘This heart is suffering from disease.’ We cannot do any of these things
for stones: stones do not suffer from stone malfunction or stone disease;
but if we assign a function to a stone–such as being a paperweight or
projectile–we could make evaluative appraisals. To put the point
succinctly, if perhaps too crudely, a function is a cause that serves a
purpose. And the purposes have to come from somewhere; in this case, they
come from human beings. In this sense functions are
intentionality-relative and therefore mind dependent.” Searle, John.
Making the Social World: The Structure of Human Civilization. 2010. Oxford
University Press. Pp. 58-9.
“We have a capacity to create a reality by representing it as existing.
The only reality that we can so create is a reality of deontology. It is a
reality that confers rights, responsibilities, and so on. However, this is
not a trivial achievement because these rights, responsibilities, and so
on are the glue that holds human society together.” Searle, John. Making
the Social World: The Structure of Human Civilization. 2010. Oxford
University Press. P. 89.
“We live in a sea of human institutional facts. Much of this is invisible
to us. Just as it is hard for the fish to see the water in which they
swim, so it is hard for us to see the institutionality in which we swim.”
Searle, John. Making the Social World: The Structure of Human
Civilization. 2010. Oxford University Press. P. 90.
“I will define a status function as a function that is performed by an
object(s), person(s), or other sort of entity(ies) and which can only be
performed in virtue of the fact that the community in which the function
is performed assigns a certain status to the object, person, or entity in
question, and the function is performed in virtue of the collective
acceptance or recognition of the object, person, or entity as having that
status.” Searle, John. Making the Social World: The Structure of Human
Civilization. 2010. Oxford University Press. P. 94.
“... we can now state the general principles on which institutional
reality is created and maintained in existence. We require exactly three
primitive notions: first, collective intentionality; second, the
assignment of function; and third, a language rich enough to enable the
creation of Status Function Declarations, including constitutive rules.”
Searle, John. Making the Social World: The Structure of Human
Civilization. 2010. Oxford University Press. Pp. 100-1.
“Well, there are two radically different sorts of cases. The literal
utterance of the sentence ‘Snow is white’ counts as the making of a
statement that snow is white, simply in virtue of its meaning. No further
speech act is necessary. But when we count pieces of paper of a particular
sort as twenty-dollar bills we are making them twenty-dollar bills by
Declaration. The Declaration makes something the case by counting it as,
that is, by declaring it to be, the case. It is essential to understand
this asymmetry to understand both the nature of language and the nature of
institutional reality.” Searle, John. Making the Social World: The
Structure of Human Civilization. 2010. Oxford University Press. P. 101.
“... the presence or absence of reciprocity is fundamentally about the
equality of relationships. The degree of transitivity is about the
internal organization of some set of relationships. And the degree of
preferential attachment is about the degree to which individuals become
differentiated from one another within the group.” Martin, John L. Social
Structures. 2009. Princeton University Press. P. 70.
“Other animals use rituals, of course, but these tend to be neurologically
hard-wired, and confined to very basic adaptive behaviors such as
courtship, mating, sexual combat, defense of territories, and other
critical activities. In contrast, humans need to use rituals at each and
every episode of interaction and to employ them continuously during the
course of an interaction in order to sustain focus, emotional moods, and
solidarity. Herbert Spencer and Emile Durkheim recognized this fact of
social life a long time ago, and most interpersonal theories build some
model of ritual activity into their respective conceptualizations, but
none seem to wonder why rituals would have to be so pervasive and
prominent in human interaction.
The answer lies, I believe, in our ape ancestry’s penchant for weak ties,
mobility, autonomy, and low sociality. Although hominid brains were
probably altered in order to increase the ability to mobilize a greater
variety of emotions, selection did not wipe out more primal propensities
for low sociality. Rather, evolutionary evidence suggests that the
expanding capacity to mobilize emotional energy and to use rituals to
assure that the requisite level of energy is generated was only laid over
older propensities for lower levels of sociality and solidarity. Humans
are thus of two minds, as it were, one pushing us to use rituals to
mobilize emotional energy, the other asserting the tendencies of our ape
ancestors.” Turner, Jonathan. On the Origins of Human Emotions: A
Sociological Inquiry into the Evolution of Human Affect. 2000. Stanford
University Press. Pp. 44-5.
“George Herbert Mead’s concept of role taking captures the essence of how
selection was channeled: significant symbols or gestures emitted by others
are used by individuals to assume their perspective in order to better
coordinate their actions with these others. Ralph H. Turner’s concept of
role making captures the reciprocal of this process: gestures emitted by
an individual consciously and unconsciously signal to others a line of
conduct, as well as the dispositions and moods associated with this
conduct. Role making is thus the stuff that makes role taking possible. An
increased ability to role-make and role-take was the key for low-sociality
animals in desperate need to get organized with others and to reveal more
permanent interpersonal attachments, for only in this way could hominids
become more attuned to each other.” Turner, Jonathan. On the Origins of
Human Emotions: A Sociological Inquiry into the Evolution of Human Affect.
2000. Stanford University Press. P. 46.
“Thus, if negative sanctions were to produce associative bonds, it was
necessary to wire the brain to produce varied and complex emotions that
could take away the dissociative effects of negative sanctioning.” Turner,
Jonathan. On the Origins of Human Emotions: A Sociological Inquiry into
the Evolution of Human Affect. 2000. Stanford University Press. P. 48.
“Of particular importance for social bonding and solidarity is the emotion
of pride, which consists mostly of happiness with self shadowed by fear
that one might not be able to act in ways that make one proud.” Turner,
Jonathan. On the Origins of Human Emotions: A Sociological Inquiry into
the Evolution of Human Affect. 2000. Stanford University Press. P. 49.
“Pride is especially significant because it is tied to an individual’s
feelings about self as an object; and pride comes when expectations
imposed by self and others have been met or exceeded. Positive sanctions
are essential to the production of pride, but once activated, pride has
the capacity to push individuals to meet expectations in the future and to
secure the positive sanctions that come with such efforts. Moreover, pride
makes individuals act in ways that forge positive social bonds, since
pride is basically happiness about self, an emotion that tends to be
contagious.” Turner, Jonathan. On the Origins of Human Emotions: A
Sociological Inquiry into the Evolution of Human Affect. 2000. Stanford
University Press. Pp. 49-50.
“Once the brain could generate variants and combinations of such primary
emotions as satisfaction-happiness, aversion-fear, assertion-anger, and
disappointment-sadness, sanctioning took on entirely new dimensions among
our hominid ancestors. Negative sanctions could now avoid disruptive
anger-fear-anger cycles and, instead, rely on less volatile affective
states like shame, guilt, sorrow, regret, and other emotions built from a
base of sadness. Positive sanctions could be increasingly used to heighten
cycles of associative affect, for social solidarity among humans cannot be
produced without high levels of positive sanctioning. And sadness could be
used to mobilize self to seek positive affect from others or to signal to
others needs for more positive sanctions.” Turner, Jonathan. On the
Origins of Human Emotions: A Sociological Inquiry into the Evolution of
Human Affect. 2000. Stanford University Press. P. 51.
“If we visualize a relatively low-sociality animal trying to get organized
on the African savanna, what would selection have to do to the
neuroanatomy of this animal? First, selection would have to enhance this
neuroanatomy so that this animal would be alert and highly attuned to the
expectations of others. Second, it would have to increase sensitivity to a
fuller range of emotions emitted by others. Third, it would have to
connect emotions and expectations together in some way so as to generate
more general codes (i.e., norms, values) of conduct. The primary emotions
of fear, anger, and satisfaction are sufficient to get this process
started, and it can be hypothesized that selection expanded these primary
emotions into variants in order to give moral codes, and the emotions on
which they are built, more complexity and subtlety. Thus, when moral codes
were violated, anger towards the violator could be aroused and used by
others to demand conformity; and reciprocally, the fear aroused in the
violator would mobilize efforts at conformity. When moral codes were
obeyed, satisfaction-happiness toward the conformer would provide positive
reinforcement for continued conformity. As more subtle variants and
combinations of these primary emotions evolved, the codes themselves and
the sanctions could correspondingly become more complex, allowing for more
flexible social arrangements in tune with hominids’ ape ancestry.” Turner,
Jonathan. On the Origins of Human Emotions: A Sociological Inquiry into
the Evolution of Human Affect. 2000. Stanford University Press. Pp. 52-3.
“Indeed, it could be argued that most codes among hominids were
prescriptive, with relatively few negative sanctions; proscriptive codes
and negative sanctions are more typical, I believe, of more complex
patterns of social organization that emerge after hunting-gathering.
Hunter-gatherers–the basic social form that sustained hominids–do not
evidence large inventories of prohibitions or much use of negative
sanctions; rather, these simple social structures are built primarily
around allowing individuals considerable personal autonomy and, at the
same time, pulling individuals together in sufficiently structured bands
organized by prescriptions backed mostly by low-key positive sanctions or
low-intensity negative sanctions such as sarcasm, pointed joking, and
ridicule.” Turner, Jonathan. On the Origins of Human Emotions: A
Sociological Inquiry into the Evolution of Human Affect. 2000. Stanford
University Press. P. 56.
“Without shame and guilt, morality has no internal footing within
individuals’ cognitive frames and feelings about themselves; in the
absence of shame and guilt, then, social control would depend upon
constant monitoring and external sanctioning of individuals by others. In
contrast, emotions like guilt and shame, especially when they operate in
conjunction with pride for having behaved competently and for meeting
expectations, locate morality inside the individual and make the
individual self-monitoring and self-sanctioning in ways that promote
social solidarity–while avoiding the dissociative costs to the group of
negative sanctioning.” Turner, Jonathan. On the Origins of Human Emotions:
A Sociological Inquiry into the Evolution of Human Affect. 2000. Stanford
University Press. P. 82.
“The individual is the precipitate of past interactional situations and an
ingredient of each new situation. An ingredient, not the determinant,
because a situation is an emergent property.” Collins, Randall.
Interaction Ritual Chains. 2004. Princeton University Press. P. 5.
“The term [ritual] has been used in roughly the fashion that I will
emphasize by some sociologists, notably Emile Durkheim and his most
creative follower in micro-sociology, Erving Goffman: that is, ritual is a
mechanism of mutually focused emotion and attention producing a
momentarily shared reality, which thereby generates solidarity and symbols
of group membership.” Collins, Randall. Interaction Ritual Chains. 2004.
Princeton University Press. P. 7.
“Deference is what individuals do toward others; demeanor is the other
side of the interaction, the construction of social self.... ... Demeanor
is a form of action, the work that he [Goffman] calls ‘face work.’ It is
not merely one-sided action, but reciprocal. The actor acquires a face or
social self in each particular situation, to just the extent that the
participants cooperate to carry off the ritual sustaining the definition
of the situational reality and who its participants are. There is
reciprocity between deference and demeanor.” Collins, Randall. Interaction
Ritual Chains. 2004. Princeton University Press. P. 19.
“Thus Goffman drew upon his fieldwork that was carried out incognito in
the schizophrenic wards of a mental hospital to make the point that one
becomes labeled as mentally ill because one persistently violates minor
standards of ritual propriety. He went on to draw the irony that mental
patients are deprived of backstage privacy, props for situational
self-presentation, and most of the other resources by which people under
ordinary conditions are allowed to show their well-demeaned selves and
their ability to take part in the reciprocity of giving ritual deference
to others.” Collins, Randall. Interaction Ritual Chains. 2004. Princeton
University Press. P. 20.
“Goffman was concerned with sophisticated deviants for the same reason. He
studied confidence artists because these are professionals attuned to the
vulnerabilities of situations, and their techniques point up the details
of the structures of normalcy that they take advantage of in order to
cheat their victims. He analyzed spies and counterespionage agents because
these are specialists in contriving, and in seeing through, an impression
of normalcy; the fine grain of normal appearances becomes plainer when one
sees a secret agent tripped up by minor details.” Collins, Randall.
Interaction Ritual Chains. 2004. Princeton University Press. P. 20.
“Once the bodies are together, there may take place a process of
intensification of shared experience, which Durkheim called collective
effervescence, and the formation of collective conscience or collective
consciousness. We might refer to it as a condition of heightened
intersubjectivity. How does this come about? Durkheim indicates two
interrelated and mutually reinforcing mechanisms:
1. Shared action and awareness: ‘[I]f left to themselves, individual
consciousnesses are closed to each other; they can communicate only by
means of signs which express their internal states. If the communication
is established between them is to become a real communion, that is to say,
a fusion of all particular sentiments into one common sentiment, the signs
expressing them must themselves be fused in one single and unique
resultant. It is the appearance of this that informs individuals that they
are in harmony and makes them conscious of their moral unity. It is by
uttering the same cry, pronouncing the same word, or preforming the same
gesture in regard to some object that they become and feel themselves to
be in unison .... Individual minds cannot come in contact and communicate
with each other except by coming out of themselves; they cannot do this
except by movements. So it is the homogeneity of these movements that
gives the group consciousness of itself .... When this homogeneity is once
established and these movements have taken a stereotyped form, they serve
to symbolize the corresponding representations. But they symbolize them
only because they have aided in forming them.’
2. Shared emotion: ‘When [the aborigines] are once come together, a sort
of electricity is formed by their collecting which quickly transports them
to an extraordinary degree of exaltation. Every sentiment expressed finds
a place without resistance in all the minds, which are very open to
outside impressions; each re-echoes the others, and is re-echoed by the
others. The initial impulse thus proceeds, growing as it goes, as an
avalanche grows in its advance. And as such active passions so free from
all control could not fail to burst out, on every side one sees nothing
but violent gestures, cries, veritable howls, and deafening noises of
every sort, which aid in intensifying still more the state of mind which
they manifest.’
“Movements carried out in common operate to focus attention, to make
participants aware of each other as doing the same thing and thus thinking
the same thing. Collective movements are signals by which
intersubjectivity is created. Collective attention enhances the expression
of shared emotion; and in turn the shared emotion acts further to
intensify collective movements and the sense of intersubjectivity.”
Collins, Randall. Interaction Ritual Chains. 2004. Princeton University
Press. P. 35. Subquotes are from Durkheim, Emile. The Elementary Forms of
Religious Life. 1912/1965. Free Press. Pp. 262-3, 247.
“What is mutually focused upon becomes a symbol of the group. In
actuality, the group is focusing on its own feeling of intersubjectivity,
its own shared emotion; but it has no way of representing this fleeting
feeling, except by representing it as embodied in an object. It reifies
its experience, makes it thing-like, and thus an emblem, treated as having
noun-like permanence. In fact, as Durkheim underlines, sentiments can only
be prolonged by symbols:
“‘Moreover, without symbols, social sentiments could have only a
precarious existence. Though very strong as long as men are together and
influence each other reciprocally, they exist only in the form of
recollections after the assembly has ended, and when left to themselves,
these become feebler and feebler; for since the group is no longer present
and active, individual temperaments easily regain the upper hand .... But
if the movements by which these sentiments are expressed are connected
with something that endures, the sentiments themselves become more
durable. These other things are constantly bringing them to mind and
arousing them; it is as though the cause which excited them in the first
place continued to act. Thus these systems of emblems, which are necessary
if society is to become conscious of itself, are no less indispensable for
assuring the continuation of this consciousness.’” Collins, Randall.
Interaction Ritual Chains. 2004. Princeton University Press. P. 37.
Subquote is from Durkheim, Emile. The Elementary Forms of Religious Life.
1912/1965. Free Press. P. 265.
“... we can see that some individuals are more privileged than others, by
being nearer to the center of the ritual than others. Rituals thus have a
double stratifying effect: between ritual insiders and outsiders; and,
inside the ritual, between ritual leaders and ritual followers.” Collins,
Randall. Interaction Ritual Chains. 2004. Princeton University Press. P.
41.
“The central mechanism of interaction ritual theory is that occasions that
combine a high degree of mutual focus of attention, that is, a high degree
of intersubjectivity, together with a high degree of emotional
entrainment–through bodily synchronization, mutual stimulation / arousal
of participants’ nervous systems–result in feelings of membership that are
attached to cognitive symbols; and result also in the emotional energy of
individual participants, giving them feelings of confidence, enthusiasm,
and desire for action in what they consider a morally proper path. These
moments of high degree of ritual intensity are high points of experience.
They are high points of collective experience, the key moments of history,
the times when significant things happen.” Collins, Randall. Interaction
Ritual Chains. 2004. Princeton University Press. P. 42.
“Interaction ritual theory provides a theory of individual motivation from
one situation to the next. Emotional energy is what individuals seek;
situations are attractive or unattractive to them to the extent that the
interaction ritual is successful in providing emotional energy. This gives
us a dynamic microsociology, in which we trace situations and their pull
or push for individuals who come into them. Note the emphasis: the
analytical starting point is the situation, and how it shapes individuals;
situations generate and regenerate the emotions and the symbolism that
charge up individuals and send them from one situation to another.”
Collins, Randall. Interaction Ritual Chains. 2004. Princeton University
Press. P. 44.
“In sum, there are several distinctive ways in which symbols circulate and
prolong group membership beyond ephemeral situations of emotional
intensity. One is as objects that are in the focus of attention of
emotionally entrained but otherwise anonymous crowds. The second is as
symbols built up out of personal identities and narratives, in
conversational rituals marking the tie between the conversationalists and
the symbolic objects they are talking about. These symbols generally
operate in two quite different circuits of social relationships;
typically, the symbols of audiences, fans, partisans, and followers
circulate from one mass gathering to another, and tend to fade in the
interim; the symbols of personal identities and reputations are the small
change of social relationships, generally of lesser momentary intensity
than audience symbols but used so frequently and in self-reinforcing
networks so as to permeate their participants’ sense of reality.” Collins,
Randall. Interaction Ritual Chains. 2004. Princeton University Press. P.
87.
“Garfinkel’s most important contribution is to show that humans have
intrinsically limited cognitive capabilities, and that they construct
mundane social order by consistently using practices to avoid recognizing
how arbitrarily social order is actually put together. We keep up
conventions, not because we believe in them, but because we studiously
avoid questioning them.” Collins, Randall. Interaction Ritual Chains.
2004. Princeton University Press. Pp. 103-4. Reference is to Garfinkel,
Harold. Studies in Ethnomethodology. 1967. Prentice-Hall.
Garfinkel’s mundane reality, for example, is characterized by the
feeling–I stress that this is a feeling rather than an explicit
cognition–that ‘nothing out of the ordinary is happening here.’ This is an
uninteresting emotion, from the point of view of the actor; but if
Garfinkel is right, considerable work went into producing that feeling of
ordinariness, and, into keeping ourselves from seeing that work itself.
Mundane reality is a members’ accomplishment.” Collins, Randall.
Interaction Ritual Chains. 2004. Princeton University Press. P. 106.
Reference is to Garfinkel, Harold. Studies in Ethnomethodology. 1967.
Prentice-Hall.
“Order-takers nevertheless are required to be present at order-giving
rituals, and are required to give at least ‘ritualistic’ assent at that
moment. They and their boss mutually recognize each other’s position, and
who has the initiative in the ritual enactment. Power rituals thus are an
asymmetrical variant on Durkheimian interactions rituals.” Collins,
Randall. Interaction Ritual Chains. 2004. Princeton University Press. P.
113.
“In what ways can individuals differ in their status group participation?
Here we need to tease apart four aspects. Two of these are characteristics
of the micro-situation itself and the individual’s location within it. Two
are meso-level characteristics of the interaction ritual chains: what
happens over time as situations repeat.
“First, on the micro-level, we must ask, How successful is the interaction
ritual? In other words, does it build up to a high level of collective
effervescence, a moderate level, or little emotional entrainment at all?
The higher the ritual intensity, the more emotion is generated both in the
immediate present and for long-term effects. Ritual intensity thus
operates as a multiplier for the other three aspects of ritual effects.
“Again, on the micro-level: Where is the individual located as the
interaction ritual takes place? There is a continuum from persons who are
on the fringes of the group, just barely members, barely participating;
others nearer the core; at the center is the sociometric star, the person
who is always most intensely involved in the ritual interaction. This
person is the Durkheimian participant of the highest degree, and
experiencing the strongest effects of ritual membership: emotional energy,
moral solidarity, attachment to group symbols. At the other end, there is
the Durkheimian nonmember, who receives no emotional energy, no moral
solidarity, and no symbolic attachments. This is the dimension of central
/ peripheral participation.
“Next, on the meso-level, as interaction ritual chains string situations
together: What proportion of their time do people spend in each other’s
physical presence? This is the dimension of social density. At one end of
the continuum individuals are always in other people’s presence, under
their eyesight and in their surveillance; this leads to a high degree of
conformity, a feeling of social pressure on oneself, but also a desire to
make other people conform as well. At the other end of the continuum
individuals have a great deal of privacy (social and physical spaces where
others do not intrude; Goffmanian backstages) or of solitude (other people
are simply not around). Here pressures for conformity are low. Social
density is a quantitative matter, an aggregate of a chain of situations
over time....”
“Again on the meso-level: Who are the participants who come together in
the aggregate of interaction ritual chains? Is it always the same persons,
or a changing cast of characters? This is the dimension of localism /
cosmopolitanism. Specifying the argument of Durkheim’s Division of Labor
in Society, low diversity should produce local solidarity, strong
attachment to reified symbols, literal-mindedness, and a strong barrier
between insiders and outsiders. There is high conformity within the group,
along with strong distrust of outsiders and alien symbols. At the other
end of this subdimension, there is participation in a loose network
consisting of many different kinds of groups and situations. Durkheimian
theory predicts the result of cosmopolitan network structure is
individualism, relativistic attitudes toward symbols, abstract rather than
concrete thinking.” Collins, Randall. Interaction Ritual Chains. 2004.
Princeton University Press. Pp. 115-7.
“Pride is the social attunement emotion, the feeling that one’s self fits
naturally into the flow of interaction, indeed that one’s personal sense
epitomizes the leading mood of the group. High solidarity is
smooth-flowing rhythmic coordination in the micro-rhythms of
conversational interaction; it gives the feeling of confidence that what
one is doing, the rewarding experience that one’s freely expressed
impulses are being followed, are resonated and amplified by the other
people present. When Scheff speaks of shame as the broken social bond, I
take this to mean that the rhythm is impaired, that one’s spontaneous
utterances are choked off–even for fractions of seconds–that there is a
hesitancy about whether one is going to be understood, and hence about
whether it is possible to formulate a clear or understandable utterance at
all.” Collins, Randall. Interaction Ritual Chains. 2004. Princeton
University Press. P. 120.
“Interaction ritual chains often have a circular, self-perpetuating form.
Persons who dominate rituals gain emotional energy, which they can use to
dominate future interaction rituals. Persons who are at the center of
attention gain emotional energy, which they can use to convene and
energize still further gatherings, thereby making themselves yet again the
center of attention.” Collins, Randall. Interaction Ritual Chains. 2004.
Princeton University Press. P. 131.
“We may visualize the stratification of society, not as a matter of who
owns what material resources, or occupies what abstract position in a
social structure, but as an unequal distribution of emotional energy.”
Collins, Randall. Interaction Ritual Chains. 2004. Princeton University
Press. P. 131.
“Emotional energy generated by experience of group solidarity is the
primary good in social interaction, and all such value-oriented behaviors
are rationally motivated toward optimizing this good. Since interaction
rituals vary in the amount of solidarity they provide, and in their costs
of participating, there is a market for ritual participation that shapes
the distribution of individual behavior.” Collins, Randall. Interaction
Ritual Chains. 2004. Princeton University Press. P. 145.
“How does one apply a rationality model to interaction ritual-based
emotional solidarity? Solidarity is a good; and individuals are motivated
to maximize the amount of solidarity they can receive, relative to costs
of producing it. Solidarity, however, is a collective good; it can only be
produced cooperatively. But it is a fairly simple type of collective
structure. Interaction rituals are not subject to the free rider problem.”
Collins, Randall. Interaction Ritual Chains. 2004. Princeton University
Press. P. 148.
“All these activities involve gradations of skill, which are typically
remarked upon by those present and recycled afterward in conversations and
thus become part of individuals’ social reputations; some persons are
better dancers, better singers, better players at a game whether it be
bridge, nineteenth-century English cricket, or twentieth-century American
pickup basketball games. Skill in such activities is part of the stock of
membership symbols, reminding us that symbols are not things or even
merely cognitions, but ways of communicating membership.” Collins,
Randall. Interaction Ritual Chains. 2004. Princeton University Press. P.
154.
“Another way to say this is that positions in networks are created and
sustained on the micro-level by the degree of success of interaction
rituals. Networks are not fixed, although it is convenient for us as
network analysts to treat them as fixed and preexisting so that we can
examine the effects of being in different network positions. Network ties
become created by just the kind of matchups of membership symbols and
emotional energies that I have been discussing. And network ties vary in
their strength, precisely as the situational ingredients that go into them
vary.” Collins, Randall. Interaction Ritual Chains. 2004. Princeton
University Press. P. 166.
“... all cases of altruism are cases of apparent conflict between
interests in social solidarity and interests in material goods (including
one’s body, seen here as a material good). If the market for interaction
rituals is the prime determinant of emotional energy, altruism is not
irrational; it is even predictable.” Collins, Randall. Interaction Ritual
Chains. 2004. Princeton University Press. Pp. 168-9.
“In interaction ritual theory, thinking is the third-order circulation of
symbols. It follows upon the first-order creation of symbols in intense
interaction rituals, and their second-order recirculation in
conversational networks. Thinking is yet another loop, now into imaginary
internal conversations, which are themselves interaction rituals taking
place in the mind. Perform a gestalt switch: instead of starting with the
individual engaged in thinking, start with the overall distribution of
symbols among a population of people. Visualize what the pattern would
look like if you could see it from the air, through a time-lapse
photography in which symbols were marked in colors, so that we could trace
where they flow, and follow their emotional energy levels as intensities
of brightness. We would see symbols circulating as streaks of light, from
person to person, and then–our camera zooming in for a close-up–flowing in
chains within a particular person’s mind.” Collins, Randall. Interaction
Ritual Chains. 2004. Princeton University Press. P. 183.
“Nouns too are Durkheimian collective symbols, for those factions of
intellectual networks that circulate them and focus on them as the center
pieces of their arguments; they are collective representations of how
groups of intellectuals see the world, and are thus the entities that are
regarded as most truly existing.” Collins, Randall. Interaction Ritual
Chains. 2004. Princeton University Press. P. 217.
“Subjectively we live in a world of symbols loaded with membership
significance, and with emotional energy levels built up in prior
interactions. Woven into the interstices between the external interaction
rituals that one goes through with other people are the inner interaction
rituals that constitute chains of thought. The guiding principle of these
inner chains, too, is emotional energy-seeking. The longer one stays
inside one’s own subjectivity in the realm of inner thought, the more the
goal becomes not so much direct solidarity with other people but
solidarity with oneself. Symbols used in inner thought become decomposed,
recombined, tried out for new purposes, aiming at imaginary coalitions not
only with persons outside but also coalitions among the parts of oneself.
Following the analogy of the intellectual thinker trying out new
combinations, the human being in private thought tries out projects,
tendering symbolic alliances that are not yet formed, entertaining mere
trajectories.” Collins, Randall. Interaction Ritual Chains. 2004.
Princeton University Press. P. 219.
“Verbal incantations–traditionally, in the form of prayers or magic;
contemporarily in the form of pep talks and curses–are just some of the
devices with which external rituals are taken into the self. No doubt
there are other such inner rituals to be discovered.” Collins, Randall.
Interaction Ritual Chains. 2004. Princeton University Press. P. 220.
“The world of thought is generally regarded as a vast territory. So it is;
but it may not be so fantastic as it is touted to be. We have a prejudice
that thought is free, untrammeled, infinitely open, unapproachable from
outside. And yet–if thought is an internalization of rituals from social
life, further developed by decomposition and recombination of its symbolic
elements, in the train of impulses to externalize them again–how strange
can it be?” Collins, Randall. Interaction Ritual Chains. 2004. Princeton
University Press. P. 220.
“Our view is that natural selection enhanced the subcortical areas of the
hominin brain before it grew the brain to the threshold where complexes of
more cortically based cultural symbols and speech could be used to forge
social bonds. In so doing, selection enhanced human emotionality–a far
easier route than is necessary for articulated speech and culture. Indeed,
if we think about the dynamics of social solidarity for just a moment,
speech and culture are far less significant for local group cohesion than
the arousal of positive emotions, per se, and their enhancement through
interpersonal attunement, rhythmic synchronization, emotional entrainment,
reciprocal exchange, positive sanctioning, and rituals.” Turner, Jonathan
& A. Maryanski. On the Origin of Societies by Natural Selection. 2008.
Paradigm. P. 87.
“What shame, guilt, and alienation accomplish is the mitigation of the
power inhering [in] anger, fear, or sadness alone. Mixing these emotions
together creates entirely new emotions, and these emotions lead
individuals to monitor and sanction self in ways that promote (1)
competent behaviors that meet expectations, (2) moral behaviors that
reaffirm the cultural codes of the group, or (3) behaviors that signal to
others that disaffection from the group exists and needs attention from
others. By rewiring late hominin neuroanatomy in this way, natural
selection reduced the power of variants and first-order combinations of
negative emotions, provided teeth for norms and other cultural codes, and
created a way for disaffections to be expressed in a non-threatening and
low-key manner. These are the emotions of social control, and they
significantly increase the power of groups over individuals.” Turner,
Jonathan & A. Maryanski. On the Origin of Societies by Natural Selection.
2008. Paradigm. P. 102.
“Thus the language that social scientists see as the hallmark of human
culture is, we argue, an adjunct to a much more ancient or hardwired
language system of emotions. Humans still rely on this system to forge
meaningful social bonds, and while these bonds are culturally embellished,
the actual mechanisms for bonding are much as they probably were millions
of years ago. As unique to humans as culture is, our emotional capacities
are equally unique. No other animal on earth can generate and understand
so many emotional states. Humans can read face and body to understand each
other’s dispositions and likely course of action without ever saying a
word, and we can easily read the one hundred or so variations on primary
emotions, as well as second-order elaborations, without verbal prompts. We
can do this because these abilities are what allowed our hominin ancestors
to survive before the brain grew and before culture could share the burden
of generating group solidarity.” Turner, Jonathan & A. Maryanski. On the
Origin of Societies by Natural Selection. 2008. Paradigm. P. 109.
“The classic differentiation here involves the organization of economic
activities into markets, hierarchies or networks. Markets are governed by
contract or property rights. Goods are exchanged on the basis of price and
participants typically seek the lowest cost supplier regardless of past
relations. Conflict tends to be resolved through bargaining and, if need
be, the law. In contrast hierarchies are defined by an employment
relationship. For the most part, employees are committed to their employer
and subject to supervision or administrative fiat. Daily routines are
conducted in the context of a mostly formal or bureaucratic system.
Networks, both within and across organizations, are based on neither
transactions nor rules, but on ongoing relationships, embedded in
friendship, obligation, reputation and possibly trust. The interdependent
and committed parties develop norms of reciprocity that lead to open-ended
relationships and mutual benefits. A network form of organizing can allow
organizations to simultaneously enjoy the benefits of being small (e.g.
responding quickly), while at the same time gaining economies of scale
that are typically reserved for much larger organizations.” Porter, Kelley
& W. Powell. “Networks and Organizations.” Pp. 776-799. From Clegg,
Steward, C. Hardy, T. Lawrence & W. Nord. The Sage Handbook of
Organization Studies, Second Edition. 2006. Sage Publications. P. 778.
“The underlying thrust of this work [research on organizational effects of
globalization challenges] indicates that hybridized control strategies and
regimes, in which elements of bureaucratic control are selectively
combined with elements of concertive control, are becoming the dominant
governance form in high value-added, service sector organizations. Within
the latter, the re-engineering of corporate culture and the fabrication of
new organizational subjectivities/identities – better aligned with the
incessant demands and endemic uncertainties of globalized competition –
emerges as the primary focus for managerial action.
“Considered in these terms, hybridization is a multi-level, systemic
process that simultaneously responds to and generates increased complexity
in organizational forms, relations and practices. Hybrids combine and
contain cultures and roles based on contradictory norms and principles by
providing mechanisms for loosely coupling competing ‘logics of collective
action’ that are required in more unstable, uncertain and competitive
environments. They tend to facilitate horizontal, rather than vertical,
decision-making processes because they have to absorb and cope with much
higher levels of contradictions, tension and conflict than would normally
be the case in simpler forms of organizing and managing.” Reed, Michael.
“Organizational Theorizing: a Historically Contested Terrain.” Pp. 19-54.
From Clegg, Steward, C. Hardy, T. Lawrence & W. Nord. The Sage Handbook of
Organization Studies, Second Edition. 2006. Sage Publications. P. 38.
“More recently, a third meta-theoretical paradigm or framework has emerged
in organization studies to challenge the ontological assumptions and
epistemological principles on which both positivism and constructionism
traded to legitimate their respective philosophical and methodological
positions. Realism – or more precisely ‘critical realism’ – has emerged as
a radical meta-theoretical alternative to both positivism and
constructionism. It maintains that ‘organization’ is necessarily embedded
in pre-existing material and social reality that fundamentally shapes the
structures and processes through which it is generated, reproduced and
transformed. This means that the epistemological principles and
theoretical practices through which we attempt to understand and explain
‘organization’ must focus on the underlying ‘real or generative’
structures and mechanisms through which the interrelated entities and
processes that constitute it are generated, sustained and changed. By
rejecting the material determinism inherent in positivism and the cultural
relativism endemic to constructionism, critical realism provides a
meta-theoretical framework in which explanatory theories and models of
historical and structural change in organizational forms and processes can
be developed.” Reed, Michael. “Organizational Theorizing: a Historically
Contested Terrain.” Pp. 19-54. From Clegg, Steward, C. Hardy, T. Lawrence
& W. Nord. The Sage Handbook of Organization Studies, Second Edition.
2006. Sage Publications. Pp. 40-1.
“For example, looking internally, Daft views organizational complexity as
proportional to the number of organizational subsystems and recommends
measuring it using three dimensions: vertical, capturing the number of
hierarchical levels; horizontal, capturing the number of units; and
geographic, capturing the number of distinct sites.” Maguire, Steve, B.
McKelvey, L. Mirabeau & N. Oztas. “Complexity Science and Organization
Studies.” Pp. 165-214. From Clegg, Steward, C. Hardy, T. Lawrence & W.
Nord. The Sage Handbook of Organization Studies, Second Edition. 2006.
Sage Publications. P. 171.
“If political change was now [after the French Revolution] to be
considered normal and sovereignty was to reside in the people, it suddenly
became imperative for everyone to understand what it was that explained
the nature and pace of change, and how the ‘people’ arrived at, could
arrive at, the decisions they were said to be making. This is the social
origin of what we later came to call the social sciences.” Wallerstein,
Immanuel. World-Systems Analysis: An Introduction. 2004. Duke University
Press. P. 4.
“New disciplines therefore grew up for this purpose [studying the present
rather than history]. There were mainly three: economics, political
science, and sociology. Why, however, would there be three disciplines to
study the present but only one to study the past? Because the dominant
liberal ideology of the nineteenth century insisted that modernity was
defined by the differentiation of three social spheres: the market, the
state, and the civil society.” Wallerstein, Immanuel. World-Systems
Analysis: An Introduction. 2004. Duke University Press. P. 6.
“The anthopologists-ethnographers studying primitive peoples and the
Orientalists studying high civilizations had one epistemological
commonality. They were both emphasizing the particularity of the group
they were studying as opposed to analyzing generic human characteristics.
Therefore they tended to feel more comfortable on the idiographic rather
than the nomothetic side of the controversy. For the most part, they
thought of themselves as being in the humanistic, hermeneutic camp of the
two-culture split rather than the science camp.” Wallerstein, Immanuel.
World-Systems Analysis: An Introduction. 2004. Duke University Press. P.
9.
“Of course, the triple set of critiques[of world-systems
theory]–world-systems rather than states as units of analysis, insistence
on the longue duree, and a unidisciplinary approach–represented an attack
on many sacred cows. It was quite expectable that there would be a
counterattack. It came, immediately and vigorously, from four camps:
nomothetic positivists, orthodox Marxists, state autonomists, and cultural
particularists.” Wallerstein, Immanuel. World-Systems Analysis: An
Introduction. 2004. Duke University Press. P. 19.
“The complex relationships of the world-economy, the firms, the states,
the households, and the trans-household institutions that link members of
classes and status-groups are beset by two opposite–but
symbiotic–ideological themes: universalism on the one hand and racism and
sexism on the other.” Wallerstein, Immanuel. World-Systems Analysis: An
Introduction. 2004. Duke University Press. P. 38.
“Is this contradictory antinomy [universalism vs. sexism and racism] a
necessary part of the modern world-system? Universalism and
anti-universalism are in fact both operative day to day, but they operate
in different arenas. Universalism tends to be the operative principle most
strongly for what we could call the cadres of the world-system–neither
those who are at the very top in terms of power and wealth, not those who
provide the large majority of the world’s workers and ordinary people in
all fields of work and all across the world, but rather an in-between
group of people who have leadership or supervisory roles in various
institutions. It is a norm that spells out the optimal recruitment mode
for such technical, professional, and scientific personnel. This
in-between group may be larger or smaller according to a country’s
location in the world-system and the local political situation. The
stronger the country’s economic position, the larger the group. Whenever
universalism loses its hold even among the cadres in particular parts of
the world-system, however, observers tend to see dysfunction, and quite
immediately there emerge political pressures (both from within the country
and from the rest of the world) to restore some degree of universalistic
criteria.” Wallerstein, Immanuel. World-Systems Analysis: An Introduction.
2004. Duke University Press. P. 40.
“On the other hand, racism, sexism, and other anti-universalistic norms
perform equally important tasks in allocating work, power, and privilege
within the world-system. They seem to imply exclusions from the social
arena. Actually they are really modes of inclusion, but of inclusion at
inferior ranks. These norms exist to justify the lower ranking, to enforce
the lower ranking, and perversely even to make it somewhat palatable to
those who have the lower ranking.” Wallerstein, Immanuel. World-Systems
Analysis: An Introduction. 2004. Duke University Press. P. 41.
“To survive in the short term, firms and larger economic systems must
efficiently exploit existing resources, including knowledge, which
requires a certain clarity and stability of goals, standards, meaning,
roles, tasks and skills. To survive in the long term they must also engage
in exploration, which entails ambiguity of meanings, and break-up of
existing standards, roles, tasks and skills. The combination of
exploitation and exploration is a paradoxical and arguably the most
important challenge for both firms and economies. In those terms, roughly
speaking the market generates exploration, firms generate exploitation and
inter-organizational alliances and networks connect the two.
“However, exploration and exploitation may be combined, to a greater or
lesser extent, within organizations.” Nooteboom, Bart. 2009. A Cognitive
Theory of the Firm: Learning, Governance and Dynamic Capabilities. Edward
Elgar Publishing. P. 3.
“As indicated, a key challenge, for organizations as well as larger
economic systems, is to combine or connect exploration and exploitation.
This entails a trade-off between problems and opportunities of cognitive
distance, seeking an optimal distance that is large enough to yield
novelty but not so large as to preclude understanding and collaboration.”
Nooteboom, Bart. 2009. A Cognitive Theory of the Firm: Learning,
Governance and Dynamic Capabilities. Edward Elgar Publishing. P. 4.
“The problems and opportunities of cognitive distance lead to the notion
of an organization as a ‘focusing device’.” Nooteboom, Bart. 2009. A
Cognitive Theory of the Firm: Learning, Governance and Dynamic
Capabilities. Edward Elgar Publishing. P. 4.
“On the basis of Hodgson I define institutions as pre-established,
prevalent, explicit rules or more implicit norms, socially transmitted,
and supported by habits, that structure, enable and constrain behaviour.
Being rules or norms, institutions have normative content or import, and
entail sanctions that may be material or immaterial, such as loss of
legitimacy, or both at the same time. Institutions are pre-established,
that is while they are constructed and re-constructed by actions, they
also precede actions and form a basis for them. They are prevalent, that
is they apply universally to members of some groups.... In the
incorporation of institutions into habit, idiosyncratic elements come in.”
Nooteboom, Bart. 2009. A Cognitive Theory of the Firm: Learning,
Governance and Dynamic Capabilities. Edward Elgar Publishing. Pp. 30-1.
Reference is to Hodgson, G. 2006. “What are institutions?” Journal of
Economic Issues. 60(1), 1-25.
“With the emergence of the first mammals some 200 million years ago,
babies were born dependent on nurture from one other individual–their
mother, who kept them safe, warm, and milk-fed. Bonds between mother and
infant were fundamental to the evolution of the ways creatures like
ourselves smell, hear, remember, sense the nearness of, and feel comforted
by those close to us. Absent mammals and minus mothers, we would not be
groping for terms to express affiliative emotions or need a word like
‘love’ to describe the ties that bind one intimate to another.” Hrdy,
Sarah. 2009. Mothers and Others: The Evolutionary Origins of Mutual
Understanding. Harvard University Press. P. 68.
“With the cortical brain expansion seen in monkeys and apes, there has
been an increase in the complexity of social relationships and a decreased
dependency on olfactory communication.” Broad, K.D., J. Curley & E.
Keverne. 2006. “Mother-infant bonding and the evolution of mammalian
social relationships.” Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society. B
2006 361, 2199-2214. P. 2200.
“The word bonding is a loosely used descriptive term to signify an
especially meaningful relationship between two or more individuals. In all
mammalian species this relationship primarily involves mother and infant.
The evolution of viviparity and the birth of live offspring as opposed to
egg production have required consolidation of the mother’s in utero
investment, resulting in extended post-natal care. This is turn has
required offspring recognition. Common to all bonding relationships are
hormonal mechanisms, brain reward mechanisms and sensory recognition.
However, the way in which these mechanisms are deployed and their relative
importance are dependent on the species and in particular on the evolution
of the brain.” Broad, K.D., J. Curley & E. Keverne. 2006. “Mother-infant
bonding and the evolution of mammalian social relationships.”
Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society. B 2006 361, 2199-2214. P.
2209.
“Eibl-Eibesfeldt sees parental care as the basis not only of group
bonding, but of individual friendship: ‘There is also, with few
exceptions, no friendship without parental care.’ He points out that
friendships are initiated by behavior that draws on the repertory of
parental care, as does, even more clearly, courtship behavior. Nuzzling,
real or pretend feeding, kissing, are all borrowed from the repertory of
parental care.” Bellah, Robert. 2011 Religion in Human Evolution. Harvard
University Press. P. 71. Subquote is from Eibl-Eibesfeldt, Irenaeus. 1996
[1971] Love and Hate: The Natural History of Behavior Patterns. Aldine. P.
127.
“Burghardt sums up by indicating five things that must in some way always
be present before we can call something animal play:
“1. Limited immediate function
2. Endogenous component
3. Structural or temporal difference
4. Repeated performance
5. Relaxed field
“The first criterion indicates that play is ‘not fully functional in the
context in which it is expressed,’ that it ‘does not contribute to current
survival.’...”
“... The second criterion is that play is something ‘done for its own
sake,’ pleasurable in itself, spontaneous and voluntary; it is not a means
to an end....”
“... The third criterion, ‘structural or temporal difference,’ indicates
that play may use behaviors from ordinary life, like fighting, chasing,
wrestling, but without the aim that such behavior would ordinarily
have....”
“...The fourth criterion is that play behavior is ‘performed repeatedly in
a similar, but not rigidly stereotyped form.’ It is, then, ‘something that
is repeatedly performed, often in bouts, during a predictable period in
the animal’s life (which in some cases can be virtually lifelong).”
“The fifth and final criterion is related to the first one: play behavior
‘is initiated when an animal is adequately fed, healthy, and free from
stress, or intense competing systems.” Bellah, Robert. 2011 Religion in
Human Evolution. Harvard University Press. P. 77. Reference is to
Burghardt, Gordon. 2005. The Genesis of Animal Play: Testing the Limits.
MIT Press. Pp. 71-81.
“Students of animal play have discerned three major forms of play:
locomotor, object, and social play. Burghardt speaks of locomotor-rotational
play as it can involve not only movement from place to place but movement
in one spot, involving various kinds of turn. This is usually the earliest
form of play in the life of the animal and is often solitary. Burghardt
gives the example of ‘the gambols of foals released from barn stalls into
a field.’ Object play is also often solitary and involves an animal
interacting with an object with no purpose other than to play. Anyone who
has ever had a cat knows what object play is, but human infants
interacting with toys is another obvious example. Social play involves at
least two animals, but sometimes more. As Burghardt says, ‘social play can
take many forms, but the most common are quasi-aggressive behavior
patterns such as chasing, wrestling, pawing, and nipping.’” Bellah,
Robert. 2011 Religion in Human Evolution. Harvard University Press. P. 78.
Subquote is from Burghardt, Gordon. 2005. The Genesis of Animal Play:
Testing the Limits. MIT Press. Pp. 83-89.
“De Waal argues that play inhibitions are probably produced by
conditioning, are ‘learned adjustments’: ‘From an early age, monkeys learn
that the fun will not last if they are too rough with a younger playmate.’
On this account play would be an expression of the plasticity and openness
to learning that arises when parental care limits the need for early
instinctive self-preserving behavior.” Bellah, Robert. 2011 Religion in
Human Evolution. Harvard University Press. P. 81. Reference is to De Waal,
Frans. 1997. Good Natured: The Origins of Right and Wrong in Humans and
Other Animals. Harvard University Press. P. 48.
“But in hominid groups that were too large for kinship alone to provide
solidarity, and that were also, perhaps, already moving away from
dominance hierarchies toward more egalitarian solidarities among both
sexes, ritual might have been just the innovation to provide the
solidarity that was necessary but not otherwise provided.
“The play features of such ritual would be evidenced in the fact that they
would be discrete events, with beginnings and ends, that they would take
place at particular times, perhaps when food was plentiful, and particular
places, perhaps some place that had significant meaning to the group.”
Bellah, Robert. 2011 Religion in Human Evolution. Harvard University
Press. P. 94.
Authors & Works cited in this section:
Baert, Patrick.
Social Theory in the Twentieth Century
Bellah, Robert. Religion in Human Evolution
Bennett, John. Human Ecology as Human Behavior; Essays in Environmental
and Developmental
Bernauer and Rasmussen, The Final Foucault
Bly, Robert, poet
Boyd, Brian. On the Evolution of Stories: Evolution, Cognition, and
Fiction
Broad, K.D. et al. “Mother-infant bonding and the evolution of mammalian
social relationships.”
Clegg, Steward, C. Hardy, T. Lawrence & W. Nord. The Sage Handbook of
Organization
Collins, Randall. Interaction Ritual Chains.
Czarniawska-Joerges, Barbara, Exploring Complex Organizations;
Elster, Jon. The Cement of Society: A Study of Social Order
Hrdy, Sarah. Mothers and Others: The Evolutionary Origins of Mutual
Understanding
Madsen, Richard et al. Meaning and Modernity: Religion, Polity
Martin, John L. Social Structures.
McAfee, Noelle. Habermas, Kristeva, and Citizenship.
Moeller, Hans-Georg. Luhmann Explained: From Souls to Systems.
Morin, Edgar. Homeland Earth: A Manifesto for a New Millennium
Nooteboom, Bart. A Cognitive Theory of the Firm: Learning, Governance and
Dynamic
Oyama, Susan et al, ed. Cycles of Contingency: Developmental Systems
Plotkin, Henry. The Imagined World Made Real: Towards a Natural
Sawyer, R. Keith. Social Emergence: Societies as Complex Systems
Schein, Edgar H., Organizational Culture and Leadership
Searle, John. Making the Social World: The Structure of Human
Civilization.
Taylor, Mark C. The Moment of Complexity: Emerging Network Culture
Tilly, Charles. Identities, Boundaries, & Social Ties
Tomasello, Michael. The Cultural Origins of Human Cognition
Turner, Jonathan. On the Origins of Human Emotions: A Sociological Inquiry
Turner, Jonathan & A. Maryanski. On the Origin of Societies by Natural
Selection.
Wallerstein, Immanuel. World-Systems Analysis: An Introduction
Watts, Duncan. Six Degrees: The Science of a Connected Age
Wheatley, Margaret, Leadership and the New Science; Learning
White, Harrison. Identity and Control: How Social Formations Emerge
Woodward, Ian. Understanding Material Culture